diff --git a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/index.md b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi.md similarity index 62% rename from data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/index.md rename to data/pages/en/resources/guaspi.md index 59ea44b67..4c0a68ae7 100644 --- a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/index.md +++ b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi.md @@ -8,14 +8,14 @@ > Abstract: _Gua\spi_ is an artificial language suited to both humans and machines. It can express real human conversation. Yet the vocabulary and the grammar are two and three orders of magnitude simpler than English. Word and phrase meanings are defined through predicate calculus and hence can be represented and manipulated efficiently and unambiguously by programs (and people). - * [Introduction to _Gua\spi_](../acmpaper) - * [_Gua\spi_ Reference Manual](../guarefmn) - * [_Gua\spi_ Dictionary](../dictintr) (introduction only, with case merge symbols) - * [_Gua\spi_ Vocabulary Lookup](../xankua) - * [TeX Documents and Miscellaneous Non-HTML Stuff](../old) + * [Introduction to _Gua\spi_](./acmpaper) + * [_Gua\spi_ Reference Manual](./guarefmn) + * [_Gua\spi_ Dictionary](./dictintr) (introduction only, with case merge symbols) + * [_Gua\spi_ Vocabulary Lookup](./xankua) + * [TeX Documents and Miscellaneous Non-HTML Stuff](./old) -If you would like to modify the vocabulary CGI script for another language, use [this link.](../xankuacgi.txt) +If you would like to modify the vocabulary CGI script for another language, use [this link.](./xankuacgi.txt) -To download the dictionary in machine readable form, use [this link.](../xankua.dat) +To download the dictionary in machine readable form, use [this link.](./xankua.dat) diff --git a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/acmpaper.md b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/acmpaper.md index a4e9c8fb6..a0b664a30 100644 --- a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/acmpaper.md +++ b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/acmpaper.md @@ -399,19 +399,19 @@ The phonemes (sounds) are divided in two classes, C's and V's, or _kona_ and _vu -> Table 2 [Pronunciation]. How to pronounce _gua\spi_ phonemes. Nonstandard C's are shown; C's without examples are as in English. Standard radio broadcast accent is close to correct for the V's; Spanish is closer. Pronounce the vowels as one sound, not a glide between two sounds as in ``eye''. +> Table 2 [Pronunciation]. How to pronounce _gua\spi_ phonemes. Nonstandard C's are shown; C's without examples are as in English. Standard radio broadcast accent is close to correct for the V's; Spanish is closer. Pronounce the vowels as one sound, not a glide between two sounds as in eye. * * * Table 1 [Phonemes] shows the phonemes, categorized by tongue position and sound type. Some phonemes are represented confusingly in English, e.g. `sh' which sounds like neither `s' nor `h'. So in _gua\spi_ they are assigned individual letters which differ from English usage --- `q' for `sh'. Table 2 [Pronunciation] gives examples of these, and all the vowels. There is a 1-1 relation between written and spoken _gua\spi_. Written blanks have no sound, and are optional. There is no distinction between upper and lower case. -All words must begin with a C and end with a V so foreign words must be modified to fit. They appear in _gua\spi_ as possibly several CnVn syllables. A prefix, most commonly _``qo''_ for a foreign name, signals that the syllables are not to be looked up in the dictionary and are to be kept together when other compound words are split up during organization. +All words must begin with a C and end with a V so foreign words must be modified to fit. They appear in _gua\spi_ as possibly several CnVn syllables. A prefix, most commonly _qo_ for a foreign name, signals that the syllables are not to be looked up in the dictionary and are to be kept together when other compound words are split up during organization. ## Grammar by Tones --- How Words Join The grammar is stated in Backus-Naur form in the Appendix. The job of grammar is to stick words together into phrases (_zdua_). _Gua\spi_ grammar produces a strict tree-structure parse for each sentence wherein one phrase is at the root forming the main sentence, and each phrase has an ordered list of zero or more sub-phrases. The grammar does not support meaning of any kind --- no tenses, no possessives, no nouns, no verbs. These ideas are handled at the organizational and semantic levels, using the grammar as a foundation. Like its morphology, the grammar of _gua\spi_ is nearly minimal. -For grammatical purposes there is only one kind of phrase (though distinctions are made at the organizational level), but words have five categories: the two grammar words _``fu''_ and _``fi''_ , sentence start prefixes, other prefixes, and everything else. The main part of a phrase is a sequence of one or more words collectively called the ``phrase predicate''; any prefixes in this must come first. During grammatical analysis there may be several content words in the predicate, though later transformations split up all compound words into separate phrases. After any of the prefixes or after the whole predicate the sub-phrases are interspersed where convenient; they attach to the current phrase at the next higher level. +For grammatical purposes there is only one kind of phrase (though distinctions are made at the organizational level), but words have five categories: the two grammar words _fu_ and _fi_ , sentence start prefixes, other prefixes, and everything else. The main part of a phrase is a sequence of one or more words collectively called the phrase predicate; any prefixes in this must come first. During grammatical analysis there may be several content words in the predicate, though later transformations split up all compound words into separate phrases. After any of the prefixes or after the whole predicate the sub-phrases are interspersed where convenient; they attach to the current phrase at the next higher level. Fig. 1 [Parsetree] shows a parse tree for a simple sentence with two levels of sub-phrases. The tones (see Table 3 [tones]) show the level of each word relative to the one before it. @@ -616,13 +616,13 @@ The tone, or frequency modulation, of a _gua\spi_ word represents its parse tree -> Table 3 [Tones]. Sounds and interpretations of the tones. ``Level'' refers to the parse tree level of the word with that tone, relative to the structure before it. ``Type'' indicates the organizational type of that word or phrase. The first set of symbols shown, ascii characters, is preferred but the second set can substitute on a manual typewriter. In this paper, `!' is used instead of `\' for convenience in typesetting. +> Table 3 [Tones]. Sounds and interpretations of the tones. Level refers to the parse tree level of the word with that tone, relative to the structure before it. Type indicates the organizational type of that word or phrase. The first set of symbols shown, ascii characters, is preferred but the second set can substitute on a manual typewriter. In this paper, `!' is used instead of `\' for convenience in typesetting. * * * Tones `-' and `=' join adjacent words of a compound phrase predicate. Tones `!' and `|' start a sub-phrase of the current phrase. Tone `^' closes the current sub-phrase and starts a new one, part of the same containing phrase. Tone `/' closes a sub-phrase and resumes the predicate of the containing phrase, if among its prefixes, or otherwise starts a new phrase at the higher level. Distinctions within these tone classes are important later but do not affect the grammar. -A sentence start prefix such as _``^:i''_ is automatically at root level, and _``/fi''_ jumps to root level without ending the sentence. Other multi-level upjumps are available with _``fu''_ but are needed rarely. +A sentence start prefix such as _^:i_ is automatically at root level, and _/fi_ jumps to root level without ending the sentence. Other multi-level upjumps are available with _fu_ but are needed rarely. ## Cases --- Members of a Relation @@ -653,9 +653,9 @@ The next layer of _gua\spi_ syntax is the organizational level, but to understan -Please pronounce it correctly: `c' as English `ch' and `i' as `ee'. Mind the tones, lest you change it into ``the eat rats the cheese'' or some such. (Chinese is worse: you could change ``mother'' into ``horse'' with a wrong tone. But Chinese people survive nicely.) The notation ``_tara_ -rat'' in examples means that _``tara''_ is the example word, and it means ``rat'' in English. Isolated words or phrases like this are written without a tone because it depends on the context where the word is used. +Please pronounce it correctly: `c' as English `ch' and `i' as `ee'. Mind the tones, lest you change it into the eat rats the cheese or some such. (Chinese is worse: you could change mother into horse with a wrong tone. But Chinese people survive nicely.) The notation _tara_ -rat in examples means that _tara_ is the example word, and it means rat in English. Isolated words or phrases like this are written without a tone because it depends on the context where the word is used. -Natural languages generally distinguish between ``things'' and ``actions'', where an ``action'' is a relation between ``things''. The formal term for such a relation is a ``predicate'' (_gna_). Take for example: +Natural languages generally distinguish between things and actions, where an action is a relation between things. The formal term for such a relation is a predicate (_gna_). Take for example: > @@ -681,11 +681,11 @@ Natural languages generally distinguish between ``things'' and ``actions'', wher -``_crw_ -eats'', called a ``predicate word'' (_qury_), is a symbol for the predicate by which the rat and the cheese are related. The predicate is like a function whose arguments are things that might be related; the value of the function is true or false (or fuzzily in between) depending on whether or not they actually are thus related: in this sentence, whether the first actual parameter eats the second. +_crw_ -eats, called a predicate word (_qury_), is a symbol for the predicate by which the rat and the cheese are related. The predicate is like a function whose arguments are things that might be related; the value of the function is true or false (or fuzzily in between) depending on whether or not they actually are thus related: in this sentence, whether the first actual parameter eats the second. -The formal parameters of a predicate, regarded as a function, are referred to as ``cases''. English has ``nominative'' and ``accusative'' cases (the rat occupies the nominative case and the cheese occupies the accusative case), and Latin has in addition ``genitive'', ``ablative'' and others, but _gua\spi_ simply numbers the cases. Some _gua\spi_ words have as many as five numbered cases. In our example, ``_tara_ -rat'' fills the first case of ``_crw_ -eats'' and ``_kseo_ -cheese'' fills the second. Natural languages and -gua!spi have obvious regularities in how particular cases are used, but it is not possible, at least in -gua!spi, to make a universal theory of what cases mean. Users should attend closely to case patterns in related words, but each category must be learned individually. +The formal parameters of a predicate, regarded as a function, are referred to as cases. English has nominative and accusative cases (the rat occupies the nominative case and the cheese occupies the accusative case), and Latin has in addition genitive, ablative and others, but _gua\spi_ simply numbers the cases. Some _gua\spi_ words have as many as five numbered cases. In our example, _tara_ -rat fills the first case of _crw_ -eats and _kseo_ -cheese fills the second. Natural languages and -gua!spi have obvious regularities in how particular cases are used, but it is not possible, at least in -gua!spi, to make a universal theory of what cases mean. Users should attend closely to case patterns in related words, but each category must be learned individually. -The words denoting the actual parameters of a predicate are called ``arguments''; being sub-phrases, they have their own predicate words. Here, _``!tara''_ and _``!kseo''_ are the arguments. The ``thing'' represented by an argument, which is the actual parameter of the sentence predicate, is something that can fill the first case of the argument's predicate. It is referred to as the ``referent'' of the argument. For example, +The words denoting the actual parameters of a predicate are called arguments; being sub-phrases, they have their own predicate words. Here, _!tara_ and _!kseo_ are the arguments. The thing represented by an argument, which is the actual parameter of the sentence predicate, is something that can fill the first case of the argument's predicate. It is referred to as the referent of the argument. For example, > @@ -717,9 +717,9 @@ While English partially segregates nouns and verbs, _gua\spi_ uses the same pred ### What Definitions Mean -In a dictionary words are defined in one or two sentences, but for _gua\spi_ these sentences are considered to be merely a learning aid. The effective definition is a set of lists of thus-related referents. For example, the referent set of ``eats'' includes a member list with our example rat in first case and our example cheese in second, as well as numerous other members containing other rats, foods, and so on ad (almost literally) infinitum. Other predicates like ``_cu_ -pair'' have referent sets that are actually infinite. +In a dictionary words are defined in one or two sentences, but for _gua\spi_ these sentences are considered to be merely a learning aid. The effective definition is a set of lists of thus-related referents. For example, the referent set of eats includes a member list with our example rat in first case and our example cheese in second, as well as numerous other members containing other rats, foods, and so on ad (almost literally) infinitum. Other predicates like _cu_ -pair have referent sets that are actually infinite. -Language users are not expected to be familiar with every object list that was, is now or ever shall be thus related. A big part of language behavior consists of the listener adding to his knowledge of which items are thus related, which information the speaker sends to him or her. Each person has his own limited experience of the world, but we speak of ``the referent set'' of a word independent of a person because words are supposed to mean the same thing to each person, and language users really do agree on meanings most of the time. +Language users are not expected to be familiar with every object list that was, is now or ever shall be thus related. A big part of language behavior consists of the listener adding to his knowledge of which items are thus related, which information the speaker sends to him or her. Each person has his own limited experience of the world, but we speak of the referent set of a word independent of a person because words are supposed to mean the same thing to each person, and language users really do agree on meanings most of the time. Humans are very good at generalizing from a few referent set members so as to recognize novel referents, and they are not satisfied with a word until they can do such a general recognition algorithm and usually come out with the same answers their neighbors do. But mechanical users of _gua\spi_ cannot be expected to show such skill, and neither can beginning human users such as infants. They must build up a referent set for a word by exhaustively hearing referent set members. If an advanced human, or advanced software, can transcend the official definition of _gua\spi_ words, that's fine --- a common (but risky) strategy for humans will be to use their native language as a guide to _gua\spi_ meanings. However, _gua\spi_ words are still defined officially in terms of referent sets simply because this definition is known to be tractable both for theory and for practical implementation. A _gua\spi_ referent set is perfectly suited to be represented as a Prolog database, if truncated to a practical size. @@ -777,7 +777,7 @@ When you speak a sentence or a subordinate assertion you do the same thing: you -your knowledge of the referent set of ``_jun_ -hunt'' includes a member which John will want to append to the ones he knows, before the cheese is stolen. This is the ultimate meaning of the _gua\spi_ sentence. +your knowledge of the referent set of _jun_ -hunt includes a member which John will want to append to the ones he knows, before the cheese is stolen. This is the ultimate meaning of the _gua\spi_ sentence. ## The Organizational Syntax Level @@ -785,13 +785,13 @@ Now that we have an unambiguous parse tree made up of phrases, what shall we do ### Which Words Go in Which Cases -The tones of grammar deliver to the organizational syntax level, for each phrase, an ordered list of attached sub-phrases, which are the arguments of the phrase predicate. For example in _``!tara /crw !kseo''_ , ``_tara_ -rat'' and ``_kseo_ -cheese'' are attached to ``_crw_ -eats'' as sub-phrases and therefore are its arguments. In the simplest and most common variation the arguments fill a sentence predicate's cases in order by number, much like English and Chinese, so ``_tara_ -rat'' fills the first case of ``_crw_ -eats'' and ``_kseo_ -cheese'' fills the second. In arguments the first case is skipped over, being filled by an invisible placeholder for the referent. This organizational syntax can be so simple because the grammar delivers unambiguous lists of arguments, whereas in English or Latin a combined syntax has to deal with both getting the arguments on the right predicates and getting them into the right cases, and so is a lot more complicated. +The tones of grammar deliver to the organizational syntax level, for each phrase, an ordered list of attached sub-phrases, which are the arguments of the phrase predicate. For example in _!tara /crw !kseo_ , _tara_ -rat and _kseo_ -cheese are attached to _crw_ -eats as sub-phrases and therefore are its arguments. In the simplest and most common variation the arguments fill a sentence predicate's cases in order by number, much like English and Chinese, so _tara_ -rat fills the first case of _crw_ -eats and _kseo_ -cheese fills the second. In arguments the first case is skipped over, being filled by an invisible placeholder for the referent. This organizational syntax can be so simple because the grammar delivers unambiguous lists of arguments, whereas in English or Latin a combined syntax has to deal with both getting the arguments on the right predicates and getting them into the right cases, and so is a lot more complicated. -The root phrase is assumed, in the absence of special cue words like _``vn''_ , to be a sentence; thus its first sub-phrase fills its first case. All sub-phrases are assumed to be arguments with empty first cases, except if they have tones or prefixed cue words that make them subordinate or infinitive clauses. +The root phrase is assumed, in the absence of special cue words like _vn_ , to be a sentence; thus its first sub-phrase fills its first case. All sub-phrases are assumed to be arguments with empty first cases, except if they have tones or prefixed cue words that make them subordinate or infinitive clauses. -Should it be inconvenient to have cases filled in order, _gua\spi_ has ways to change the order. First, certain prefixes signify that the relation word is ``converted'': one case is moved in front of the others. This is most useful for arguments, and lets one _gua\spi_ word do the job of as many as five English words. For example in _``zu -crw''_ the second case comes first, and the referent of such an argument would be something occupying the second case of ``eats'' before conversion: the meaning is ``food''. The original first case, the eater, comes afterward: _``zu -crw !xo -tara''_ means ``rat food''. The English ``passive voice'' is a conversion in a sentence. +Should it be inconvenient to have cases filled in order, _gua\spi_ has ways to change the order. First, certain prefixes signify that the relation word is converted: one case is moved in front of the others. This is most useful for arguments, and lets one _gua\spi_ word do the job of as many as five English words. For example in _zu -crw_ the second case comes first, and the referent of such an argument would be something occupying the second case of eats before conversion: the meaning is food. The original first case, the eater, comes afterward: _zu -crw !xo -tara_ means rat food. The English passive voice is a conversion in a sentence. -Second, an argument can be directed to a specific case by a ``caselink'' prefix analogous to the case endings of Latin. For example, take +Second, an argument can be directed to a specific case by a caselink prefix analogous to the case endings of Latin. For example, take > @@ -818,17 +818,17 @@ Second, an argument can be directed to a specific case by a ``caselink'' prefix -_``fer''_ means ``X1 carries X2 to X3 from X4 via X5''. Its arguments are _``!tara''_ in the first case, but _``se''_ links the next argument, ``_dowu_ -house'', to the fourth case: the start point. +_fer_ means X1 carries X2 to X3 from X4 via X5. Its arguments are _!tara_ in the first case, but _se_ links the next argument, _dowu_ -house, to the fourth case: the start point. -A predicate word can act as a caselink too, linking a ``modal case'' by means of an imbedded sentence. Such sentences are covered in the next two sections. +A predicate word can act as a caselink too, linking a modal case by means of an imbedded sentence. Such sentences are covered in the next two sections. -The motion words have complicated definitions, so all the definitions have been made similar: ``X1 (moves) to X2 from X3 via X4'' or ``X1 makes X2 (move) to X3 from X4 via X5''. Many other word categories have uniform definitions too. +The motion words have complicated definitions, so all the definitions have been made similar: ``X1 (moves) to X2 from X3 via X4'' or X1 makes X2 (move) to X3 from X4 via X5. Many other word categories have uniform definitions too. ### Sentences as Arguments --- Infinitives -A _gua\spi_ sentence or argument expresses a relation between specific referents, and this specific referent set member is called an ``event''. (Frequently the sentence represents several similar events.) It is common for several cases of the predicate to be vacant: in the previous example the thing carried, the destination and the route were not specified. Nonetheless there must have been a thing carried, a destination and a route, and the sentence asserts a relation between all five arguments. The next organizational elements we will look at are linking words that attach sentence predicates (with their arguments). The linked sentences represent lists of specific events with specific argument referents and with all cases filled even if not specified by words. +A _gua\spi_ sentence or argument expresses a relation between specific referents, and this specific referent set member is called an event. (Frequently the sentence represents several similar events.) It is common for several cases of the predicate to be vacant: in the previous example the thing carried, the destination and the route were not specified. Nonetheless there must have been a thing carried, a destination and a route, and the sentence asserts a relation between all five arguments. The next organizational elements we will look at are linking words that attach sentence predicates (with their arguments). The linked sentences represent lists of specific events with specific argument referents and with all cases filled even if not specified by words. -Returning to organization, the first sentence link word is _``vo''_ , which acts to change a sentence into a one-argument predicate, referred to as an ``infinitive'', which means that the occupant of its first case is an instance of the sentence relation. Though the infinitive can itself be a sentence predicate it is much more commonly used in arguments, like this: +Returning to organization, the first sentence link word is _vo_ , which acts to change a sentence into a one-argument predicate, referred to as an infinitive, which means that the occupant of its first case is an instance of the sentence relation. Though the infinitive can itself be a sentence predicate it is much more commonly used in arguments, like this: > @@ -854,9 +854,9 @@ Returning to organization, the first sentence link word is _``vo''_ , which acts -_``vyu''_ means ``X1 enjoys doing (_vo_) X2'', where the second case is some kind of activity --- a natural place to fill with an infinitive. The sentence linked by _``vo''_ is _``!tara /crw''_ = ``the rat eats'', and an instance of that relation, an event, is the referent of the argument _``vo -crw !tara''_. (The argument _``!tara''_ may come before or after the sentence predicate _``crw''_ , wherever convenient.) +_vyu_ means X1 enjoys doing (_vo_) X2, where the second case is some kind of activity --- a natural place to fill with an infinitive. The sentence linked by _vo_ is _!tara /crw_ = the rat eats, and an instance of that relation, an event, is the referent of the argument _vo -crw !tara_. (The argument _!tara_ may come before or after the sentence predicate _crw_ , wherever convenient.) -_``vyu''_ includes the prefix _``vo''_ on its second case by default, as do all words which commonly have infinitive arguments. Also, there are various patterns in which main sentence arguments are replicated into infinitives, and by far the most common is for the argument just before the infinitive to be replicated, if the infinitive has none --- _``!tara''_ here. So you could just say +_vyu_ includes the prefix _vo_ on its second case by default, as do all words which commonly have infinitive arguments. Also, there are various patterns in which main sentence arguments are replicated into infinitives, and by far the most common is for the argument just before the infinitive to be replicated, if the infinitive has none --- _!tara_ here. So you could just say > @@ -884,7 +884,7 @@ _``vyu''_ includes the prefix _``vo''_ on its second case by default, as do all ### Subordinate Clauses -A subordinate clause is a sentence within a sentence. Its predicate relates one (or more) of its internal arguments to the phrase it modifies; the internal argument is called a ``modal case'' of the modified phrase. A subordinate clause can specify a tense, location, possession (genitive case), listener (vocative case), speaker in dialogue, gender, plural number, repeated action, and numerous miscellaneous cases as in the examples below. Its purpose is signalled by a linking word: +A subordinate clause is a sentence within a sentence. Its predicate relates one (or more) of its internal arguments to the phrase it modifies; the internal argument is called a modal case of the modified phrase. A subordinate clause can specify a tense, location, possession (genitive case), listener (vocative case), speaker in dialogue, gender, plural number, repeated action, and numerous miscellaneous cases as in the examples below. Its purpose is signalled by a linking word: _ve_ A supplementary comment, giving additional information about the modified phrase, typically adding a modal case. @@ -897,12 +897,12 @@ _va_ Subordinate clauses are more common in _gua\spi_ than in English, and also can be complicated, so two special rules are provided to make them simpler: - * Because subordinate clauses are so common the tone `|' is allocated specifically to them which automatically supplies the linking prefix _``ve''_. When this tone does not apply, of course, _``ve''_ may be used explicitly. `|' is used commonly with the other linking words too. - * The restricted phrase is automatically replicated via a placeholder pronoun into whichever case of the subordinate clause is intended for an event, indicated by default _``vo''_ or _``bi''_ , or the first case if no event is expected or if the event case is occupied. + * Because subordinate clauses are so common the tone `|' is allocated specifically to them which automatically supplies the linking prefix _ve_. When this tone does not apply, of course, _ve_ may be used explicitly. `|' is used commonly with the other linking words too. + * The restricted phrase is automatically replicated via a placeholder pronoun into whichever case of the subordinate clause is intended for an event, indicated by default _vo_ or _bi_ , or the first case if no event is expected or if the event case is occupied. -Here is a subordinate clause restricting an argument, illustrating _``vu''_ : +Here is a subordinate clause restricting an argument, illustrating _vu_ : > @@ -928,7 +928,7 @@ Here is a subordinate clause restricting an argument, illustrating _``vu''_ : -Not all cheeses but only those which smell (_|xel_) are eaten by the rat. The restricting sentence is ``X1 smells like X2'', and argument referents (cheeses) are automatically brought into X1 through the placeholder. When in English we use adjectives and adverbs, in _gua\spi_ we usually use subordinate clauses like this one. Here are examples of the other subordinate linking words: +Not all cheeses but only those which smell (_|xel_) are eaten by the rat. The restricting sentence is X1 smells like X2, and argument referents (cheeses) are automatically brought into X1 through the placeholder. When in English we use adjectives and adverbs, in _gua\spi_ we usually use subordinate clauses like this one. Here are examples of the other subordinate linking words: > @@ -954,7 +954,7 @@ Not all cheeses but only those which smell (_|xel_) are eaten by the rat. The re -This subordinate clause adds a modal case. The clause is _``!vden !xdro !fu -tum !vo (event)''_ = ``its teeth are the tool for doing (event)'', and the asserted relation _``!tara /crw !kseo''_ = ``The rat eats the cheese'' also satisfies this sub-sentence. Because of the clause we know that the rat does not gum the cheese. The effect is as if an additional case were added to ``_crw_ -eat'' for the cutting tool. +This subordinate clause adds a modal case. The clause is _!vden !xdro !fu -tum !vo (event)_ = its teeth are the tool for doing (event), and the asserted relation _!tara /crw !kseo_ = The rat eats the cheese also satisfies this sub-sentence. Because of the clause we know that the rat does not gum the cheese. The effect is as if an additional case were added to _crw_ -eat for the cutting tool. > @@ -980,7 +980,7 @@ This subordinate clause adds a modal case. The clause is _``!vden !xdro !fu -tum -_``|vi -bwy''_ appears to have no modal argument, but for this discursive category a pronoun is provided by default to represent the previous sentence. Thus the subordinate clause says ``this sentence differs from the previous one''. The other clause beginning with _``va''_ is a subordinate assertion, which is similar to a main sentence, but the reader can understand it better when it is imbedded. +_|vi -bwy_ appears to have no modal argument, but for this discursive category a pronoun is provided by default to represent the previous sentence. Thus the subordinate clause says ``this sentence differs from the previous one''. The other clause beginning with _va_ is a subordinate assertion, which is similar to a main sentence, but the reader can understand it better when it is imbedded. > @@ -1006,21 +1006,21 @@ _``|vi -bwy''_ appears to have no modal argument, but for this discursive catego -In ``_tan_ -annoy'' who is annoyed? ``_ji_ -me'' is provided by default in the first case (after conversion) of supplementary assertions, main phrases and infinitives that otherwise lack one (provided certain conditions are met). Forceful or emotional speech seems more free and expressive with this feature. The other clause _``|zey !ji''_ is a possessive phrase; most languages have special grammar just for possessives, but _gua\spi_ uses the general clause mechanism. +In _tan_ -annoy who is annoyed? _ji_ -me is provided by default in the first case (after conversion) of supplementary assertions, main phrases and infinitives that otherwise lack one (provided certain conditions are met). Forceful or emotional speech seems more free and expressive with this feature. The other clause _|zey !ji_ is a possessive phrase; most languages have special grammar just for possessives, but _gua\spi_ uses the general clause mechanism. ### Pronouns Represent Words, Not Things -The next organizational issue is the pronoun. English pronouns have referents just like any other argument. But _gua\spi_ pronouns represent words, not the referent of words. In computer terms, they are like functions that are expanded in-line rather than being called. The represented words are called the ``antecedent'' of the pronoun, and the sentence is analysed as if each pronoun were taken out and replaced by its antecedent. The antecedents, not the pronouns, have referents. In this way the organizational syntax level can be kept free of meaning, and the semantic level has to deal with only one class of words, predicates. +The next organizational issue is the pronoun. English pronouns have referents just like any other argument. But _gua\spi_ pronouns represent words, not the referent of words. In computer terms, they are like functions that are expanded in-line rather than being called. The represented words are called the antecedent of the pronoun, and the sentence is analysed as if each pronoun were taken out and replaced by its antecedent. The antecedents, not the pronouns, have referents. In this way the organizational syntax level can be kept free of meaning, and the semantic level has to deal with only one class of words, predicates. -For example, a document typically will have a signature line saying in effect ``this text is the output of Jim Carter''. (Spoken discourse is analogous though identification is by sight or voice tone.) Then when there appears the pronoun _``ji''_ (``me'' in English) the effect is as if the words ``Jim Carter'' had been written in its place. That is, ``A rat ate my cheese'' and ``A rat ate Jim Carter's cheese'' mean exactly the same thing. +For example, a document typically will have a signature line saying in effect this text is the output of Jim Carter. (Spoken discourse is analogous though identification is by sight or voice tone.) Then when there appears the pronoun _ji_ (me in English) the effect is as if the words Jim Carter had been written in its place. That is, A rat ate my cheese and A rat ate Jim Carter's cheese mean exactly the same thing. As illustrated below, various kinds of context are carried into a phrase by a pronoun-like mechanism. When an antecedent is replicated to replace a pronoun the context is replicated with it, so the antecedent will have the same referent in both places despite intervening context changes. And when the antecedent is copied any pronouns originally in it have already been replaced by their own antecedents. -_Gua\spi_ includes question pronouns, phrase-relative pronouns, names and modal pronouns. For question pronouns the listener is supposed to say the antecedent; in other words, the speaker provides a sentence and the listener is to fill in the blanks. There are question pronouns corresponding to English ``who'', ``how'', ``how many'', ``which'' and ``isn't it''. +_Gua\spi_ includes question pronouns, phrase-relative pronouns, names and modal pronouns. For question pronouns the listener is supposed to say the antecedent; in other words, the speaker provides a sentence and the listener is to fill in the blanks. There are question pronouns corresponding to English who, how, how many, which and isn't it. Phrase-relative pronouns are for copying neighboring phrases --- arguments or entire sentences. One of the more common phrase-relative pronouns is _vgry_ , the whole phrase that the current listener last said, which typically is the question the speaker is filling blanks in. -In _gua\spi_ a name is a pronoun. A name consists of a predicate prefixed by _``qu''_ , or _``qo''_ for foreign names, which disconnects the usual meaning of the predicate and substitutes the pronoun behavior. People are assigned permanent names at birth through a performative (ritual) statement like this: +In _gua\spi_ a name is a pronoun. A name consists of a predicate prefixed by _qu_ , or _qo_ for foreign names, which disconnects the usual meaning of the predicate and substitutes the pronoun behavior. People are assigned permanent names at birth through a performative (ritual) statement like this: > @@ -1046,11 +1046,11 @@ In _gua\spi_ a name is a pronoun. A name consists of a predicate prefixed by _`` -From then on, _``!jw |cil''_ (``this child'', with context so the listeners remember which one) is the antecedent of the name _``qo -ben''_. +From then on, _!jw |cil_ (this child, with context so the listeners remember which one) is the antecedent of the name _qo -ben_. The six variables _da, de, di, do, du, dy_ are names which you can assign to important concepts in nonfiction or characters in fiction. In mathematics it is also common to use letter words as pronouns for mathematical expressions. -Modal pronouns are like ``_ji_ -me'' and ``_jn_ -now''. A modal pronoun's antecedent is set by a modal phrase with a special prefix, saying to save the modal phrase on a kind of stack, separate for each modal predicate, from which it can be retrieved. The previous antecedent can be replaced, but of more interest, it can be saved and later restored. +Modal pronouns are like _ji_ -me and _jn_ -now. A modal pronoun's antecedent is set by a modal phrase with a special prefix, saying to save the modal phrase on a kind of stack, separate for each modal predicate, from which it can be retrieved. The previous antecedent can be replaced, but of more interest, it can be saved and later restored. The modal stack is used for more than modal pronouns, though. For each kind of modal case, e.g. tense or speaker, every sentence that lacks a modal phrase for that case gets the stacked phrase automatically. Here is an example of stacked speaker cases, in story dialog: @@ -1069,7 +1069,11 @@ The modal stack is used for more than modal pronouns, though. For each kind of m - Says Kira, ``Why do you flee?'' (default saved, set) + Says Kira, + + Why do you flee? + + (default saved, set) @@ -1079,7 +1083,10 @@ The modal stack is used for more than modal pronouns, though. For each kind of m - ``Don't you want to be rescued?'' (default inserted automatically) + + Don't you want to be rescued? + + (default inserted automatically) @@ -1089,7 +1096,10 @@ The modal stack is used for more than modal pronouns, though. For each kind of m - ``Please, at least talk to me!'' (default inserted automatically) + + Please, at least talk to me! + + (default inserted automatically) @@ -1108,15 +1118,15 @@ The modal stack is used for more than modal pronouns, though. For each kind of m -Kira said the first three sentences. In the first, _``|qe''_ indicates that the current speaker, the narrator, should be saved while Kira speaks. The words _``|jai !qo -kira''_ are added to the second and third sentences automatically. Finally, _``qa''_ restores the narrator as speaker and his modal phrase is put on automatically. +Kira said the first three sentences. In the first, _|qe_ indicates that the current speaker, the narrator, should be saved while Kira speaks. The words _|jai !qo -kira_ are added to the second and third sentences automatically. Finally, _qa_ restores the narrator as speaker and his modal phrase is put on automatically. -Tenses are handled the same way. If you put _``|qe -cnu !X''_ on the opening descriptive sentence (where X is an event identifying when the sentence happens) then it will be propagated to subsequent sentences automatically --- unlike in English where a syntactically complicated and less precise tense has to be used on every sentence. John Parks-Clifford, then with the Loglan Institute, originally developed this concept of tense defaults [TL43]. +Tenses are handled the same way. If you put _|qe -cnu !X_ on the opening descriptive sentence (where X is an event identifying when the sentence happens) then it will be propagated to subsequent sentences automatically --- unlike in English where a syntactically complicated and less precise tense has to be used on every sentence. John Parks-Clifford, then with the Loglan Institute, originally developed this concept of tense defaults [TL43]. ### Compound Predicates -A key organizational element of _gua\spi_ is the compound predicate, a sequence of words heading a phrase. The motivation to make compounds is threefold. First, you can use a single argument list to say what amounts to two sentences, which when compounded are much easier for the listener to interpret. Second, just as we use Latin prefixes in English to make many words from one, e.g. ``ob-ject'', ``pro-ject'', ``in-ject'', ``ab-ject'', most meanings in _gua\spi_ are achieved by combining a much broader range of predicates. A beginner can learn the primitive words (_qury_), about 1400, and then stick them together in self-created compounds which he can expect any listener to understand, while to achieve the same range of expression in natural languages the speaker and the listener must master a huge vocabulary in which most of the words are rarely used. Third, the compound words are deconstructed into phrases headed by one _qury_ word each, and semantically processing these phrases is much easier than in natural languages because there are so few _qury_ that must be kept track of. +A key organizational element of _gua\spi_ is the compound predicate, a sequence of words heading a phrase. The motivation to make compounds is threefold. First, you can use a single argument list to say what amounts to two sentences, which when compounded are much easier for the listener to interpret. Second, just as we use Latin prefixes in English to make many words from one, e.g. ob-ject, pro-ject, in-ject, ab-ject, most meanings in _gua\spi_ are achieved by combining a much broader range of predicates. A beginner can learn the primitive words (_qury_), about 1400, and then stick them together in self-created compounds which he can expect any listener to understand, while to achieve the same range of expression in natural languages the speaker and the listener must master a huge vocabulary in which most of the words are rarely used. Third, the compound words are deconstructed into phrases headed by one _qury_ word each, and semantically processing these phrases is much easier than in natural languages because there are so few _qury_ that must be kept track of. -There are three main patterns to the compounds. First, if the main word has a case with a default linker of _``vo''_ or _``bi''_ \--- that is, a case for an infinitive --- a word compounded with high even tone `-' is the predicate of that infinitive, and the main word case before the infinitive (before conversion) becomes the infinitive's first case. (Exceptions are noted in the dictionary.) +There are three main patterns to the compounds. First, if the main word has a case with a default linker of _vo_ or _bi_ \--- that is, a case for an infinitive --- a word compounded with high even tone `-' is the predicate of that infinitive, and the main word case before the infinitive (before conversion) becomes the infinitive's first case. (Exceptions are noted in the dictionary.) > @@ -1188,7 +1198,7 @@ There are three main patterns to the compounds. First, if the main word has a ca -Second, the words may share an argument list. The effect is as if you had made two sentences with the arguments copied into each. This pattern is cued by tone `-' when the infinitive argument pattern does not apply, or by a conjunction _``-fe''_ when it does. +Second, the words may share an argument list. The effect is as if you had made two sentences with the arguments copied into each. This pattern is cued by tone `-' when the infinitive argument pattern does not apply, or by a conjunction _-fe_ when it does. > @@ -1331,9 +1341,9 @@ That is how _gua\spi_ is organized. Let us now turn to the semantics of argument As stated earlier, a predicate word expresses a relation between the occupants of its cases, and is defined by a referent set consisting of lists of case occupants that are thus related. -To interpret an argument, you start with its predicate's referent set. You retain members consistent with any sub-phrases. From each member you extract the first case occupant (after conversion), and out of these you make the ``full referent set'' of the argument. The ``referent subset'', which is the set of actual referents of the argument, is a subset of the full set which depends on a prefix word called an ``article'' (_tirl_). (More modern terminology might be ``determiner''.) +To interpret an argument, you start with its predicate's referent set. You retain members consistent with any sub-phrases. From each member you extract the first case occupant (after conversion), and out of these you make the full referent set of the argument. The referent subset, which is the set of actual referents of the argument, is a subset of the full set which depends on a prefix word called an article (_tirl_). (More modern terminology might be determiner.) -The most common article is _``xe''_ , and it is assumed with most predicates when arguments lack an article. Its English translation is ``the''. The referent subset is whichever members the speaker has in mind to talk about, but generally there are prior context cues to show which out of numerous possibilities are intended as the referents. In particular, if a set of referents has been designated before and if it is the only such set that is a subset of the full referent set of the argument, then those are the referents of the argument. For example, +The most common article is _xe_ , and it is assumed with most predicates when arguments lack an article. Its English translation is the. The referent subset is whichever members the speaker has in mind to talk about, but generally there are prior context cues to show which out of numerous possibilities are intended as the referents. In particular, if a set of referents has been designated before and if it is the only such set that is a subset of the full referent set of the argument, then those are the referents of the argument. For example, > @@ -1369,9 +1379,9 @@ The most common article is _``xe''_ , and it is assumed with most predicates whe -``_fkar_ -car'' appears three times. The first instance designates one referent in detail using _``xo''_ , described below. The other two instances are typical arguments with ``_xe_ -the'', but the article is unseen, being provided by default. Since the prior referent fits this predicate (and in the second sentence ``its garage'' does not), the prior referent is being redesignated. Because _gua\spi_ words are so short it is just as efficient to redesignate an argument like this as to use a phrase-relative pronoun, so pronouns are less commonly used in _gua\spi_ than in English. +_fkar_ -car appears three times. The first instance designates one referent in detail using _xo_ , described below. The other two instances are typical arguments with _xe_ -the, but the article is unseen, being provided by default. Since the prior referent fits this predicate (and in the second sentence its garage does not), the prior referent is being redesignated. Because _gua\spi_ words are so short it is just as efficient to redesignate an argument like this as to use a phrase-relative pronoun, so pronouns are less commonly used in _gua\spi_ than in English. -There are also articles that select the entire referent set, ``typical'' members of it, and no members of it (actually making a negative statement about all members). Another important article is _``xo''_ : From the full referent set one or more members are selected, and it doesn't matter which ones. For example, +There are also articles that select the entire referent set, typical members of it, and no members of it (actually making a negative statement about all members). Another important article is _xo_ : From the full referent set one or more members are selected, and it doesn't matter which ones. For example, > @@ -1397,11 +1407,11 @@ There are also articles that select the entire referent set, ``typical'' members -All in the box are equivalent and it doesn't matter which you get. _``xo''_ is often used for arguments in the ``serving or portion'' category, called ``partitive nouns'' in English. +All in the box are equivalent and it doesn't matter which you get. _xo_ is often used for arguments in the ``serving or portion'' category, called partitive nouns in English. There are two articles for each meaning; the first unfolds the referent subset so each member is a referent, while the second specifies that the referent is the referent subset as a set. The careful distinction between sets and extensions of their members is characteristic of _gua\spi_. -Most Indo-European languages distinguish between genders and numbers of arguments. Like Chinese and English, _gua\spi_ has no gender, though you may use a subordinate clause like ``_|fmy_ -female''. Number comes from the referent sets, not the grammar. You may specify the exact number of referents with a numeric predicate, like this: +Most Indo-European languages distinguish between genders and numbers of arguments. Like Chinese and English, _gua\spi_ has no gender, though you may use a subordinate clause like _|fmy_ -female. Number comes from the referent sets, not the grammar. You may specify the exact number of referents with a numeric predicate, like this: > @@ -1547,7 +1557,10 @@ Here are examples of a few modal cases. However, virtually any word can be const Said - Kira, ``You're a monster'' + Kira, + + You're a monster + @@ -1593,13 +1606,13 @@ Here are examples of a few modal cases. However, virtually any word can be const In _gua\spi_ negation is not a unitary concept; beside the obvious antonyms there are nine or ten ways to express negative meanings, most of which involve compound words. Here are a few examples. _^:i _-go_ !ji /kio !tara |zey !ju_ - I _don't_ have your rat. _``go''_ is a mood prefix which means that the asserted sentence is counter to fact. + I _don't_ have your rat. _go_ is a mood prefix which means that the asserted sentence is counter to fact. _^:i !jw |kseo /fi _-gl_ -zao_ - This cheese is flavor _less_. Some dimensions like ``_zao_ -flavor'' are quantifiable (more or less) but unsigned, so their degree ranges from zero to larger values. Others like ``_gal_ -high'' are signed. In either case _``gl''_ modifies a predicate so that its degree is zero or negligible. + This cheese is flavor _less_. Some dimensions like _zao_ -flavor are quantifiable (more or less) but unsigned, so their degree ranges from zero to larger values. Others like _gal_ -high are signed. In either case _gl_ modifies a predicate so that its degree is zero or negligible. _^:i !jw |kseo /fi _-gr_ -ksi_ - This cheese is _not_ fresh. When the dimension ranges from positive to negative values, _``gr''_ interchanges positive and negative. For extremes of unfreshness one can use ``_fpu_ -rotten''. + This cheese is _not_ fresh. When the dimension ranges from positive to negative values, _gr_ interchanges positive and negative. For extremes of unfreshness one can use _fpu_ -rotten. _^:i !jw |kseo /fi _-vry_ -can -psl_ - This cheese is _de_ solidifying. ``_vry_ -reverse'' indicates that the process in its X2 case is occuring in the reverse of the usual order. + This cheese is _de_ solidifying. _vry_ -reverse indicates that the process in its X2 case is occuring in the reverse of the usual order. ### Causal Connectives @@ -1637,11 +1650,11 @@ The root structure of syntax is a discourse, or sequence of sentences. But the s -The speaker may connect sentences with any useful word having suitable cases, such as ``_kau_ -cause'', ``_kmo_ -motivate'' or ``_sny_ -imply''. Like all _gua\spi_ words, the causal connectives can also be useful as arguments and as modal caselinks. In this example _``^:o''_ is a ``retroactive downjump'', a special case in organization. A sentence start word, it transforms itself into a pronoun for the previous sentence, which goes into the first case of the following main word, the causal connective in these examples. Human speakers prefer infix causal connectives with a retroactive downjump rather than the obvious form with two explicit infinitives. +The speaker may connect sentences with any useful word having suitable cases, such as _kau_ -cause, _kmo_ -motivate or _sny_ -imply. Like all _gua\spi_ words, the causal connectives can also be useful as arguments and as modal caselinks. In this example _^:o_ is a retroactive downjump, a special case in organization. A sentence start word, it transforms itself into a pronoun for the previous sentence, which goes into the first case of the following main word, the causal connective in these examples. Human speakers prefer infix causal connectives with a retroactive downjump rather than the obvious form with two explicit infinitives. ### Mathematical Expressions -Any discussion sooner or later involves quantitative statements with units of measure. Therefore _gua\spi_ has quite an extensive facility for mathematical expressions, even if the more complicated possibilities are rarely used by non-scientists. First, _gua\spi_ syntax matches perfectly the definition of a ``number'' as an equivalence class of equal-count sets. This concept can be generalized to various extension rings and fields. +Any discussion sooner or later involves quantitative statements with units of measure. Therefore _gua\spi_ has quite an extensive facility for mathematical expressions, even if the more complicated possibilities are rarely used by non-scientists. First, _gua\spi_ syntax matches perfectly the definition of a number as an equivalence class of equal-count sets. This concept can be generalized to various extension rings and fields. > @@ -1658,7 +1671,11 @@ Any discussion sooner or later involves quantitative statements with units of me - The number 2.5 (the class of all sets of ``count'' 2.5) + The number 2.5 (the class of all sets of + + count + + 2.5) @@ -1667,7 +1684,7 @@ Any discussion sooner or later involves quantitative statements with units of me -Mathematical functions are defined with such classes as formal parameters, and hence have _``xu''_ on parameter cases by default --- _``xu''_ means the entire referent set of an argument, as a set (or class). The first case of a function is its value, and the function is defined as ``X1 is in the equivalence class that comes from doing (function) on (_xu_) X2'', possibly with several parameters. For example, +Mathematical functions are defined with such classes as formal parameters, and hence have _xu_ on parameter cases by default --- _xu_ means the entire referent set of an argument, as a set (or class). The first case of a function is its value, and the function is defined as ``X1 is in the equivalence class that comes from doing (function) on (_xu_) X2'', possibly with several parameters. For example, > @@ -1753,7 +1770,7 @@ Brackets `[ ]' surround sentence-type phrases; angle brackets `<>' mark subordin [then the boat1 and2 carry2 surprise < paragraph \surprise1\> < speaker/cry/ tiger's2 eye1 < performative name \eye1\ variable b2> \surprise2\> < listener/warn/ you2 (child) \surprise2\> < time/present/ (something2) \surprise1\>] -``Carry-boat'' is used for ``barge''. _``qe''_ sets the speaker and listener modal case antecedents. However, ``_gau_ -warn'' supercedes ``_kam_ -cry'' as the predicate bearing the listener. ``_do_ -variable~b'' is assigned to represent Tigereye. Words for document structure like ``_gza_ -paragraph'' fit naturally into the text, written or spoken. +Carry-boat is used for barge. _qe_ sets the speaker and listener modal case antecedents. However, _gau_ -warn supercedes _kam_ -cry as the predicate bearing the listener. _do_ -variable~b is assigned to represent Tigereye. Words for document structure like _gza_ -paragraph fit naturally into the text, written or spoken. > @@ -1781,7 +1798,7 @@ Brackets `[ ]' surround sentence-type phrases; angle brackets `<>' mark subordin [imperative1 (child) swim place2 (something) < listener/emphatic/ you3\,(child) \swim2\> < speaker/emphatic/ I1 (eye) \swim2\> < time/present/ (something2) \swim1\>] -_``jo''_ instead of ``_ju_ -you'' makes the sentence imperative. ``_faw_ -emphatic'' has a default ``_ji_ -me'' in the case for its speaker and ``_ju_ -you'' in the case for its listener, thus superceding the default _kam_ otherwise imported from the first sentence. +_jo_ instead of _ju_ -you makes the sentence imperative. _faw_ -emphatic has a default _ji_ -me in the case for its speaker and _ju_ -you in the case for its listener, thus superceding the default _kam_ otherwise imported from the first sentence. > @@ -1809,7 +1826,7 @@ _``jo''_ instead of ``_ju_ -you'' makes the sentence imperative. ``_faw_ -emphat [imperative1 (qo se fo) make to2 [your2 (qo se fo's) buttock1 move] < listener/emphatic/ you3 (qo se fo) \make2\> < speaker/emphatic/ I1 (eye) \make2\> < time/present/ (something2) \make1\>] -Only a few words like ``_fer_ -carry'' are intrinsically transitive. Normally an infinitive compound with ``_qma_ -make'' makes words transitive. +Only a few words like _fer_ -carry are intrinsically transitive. Normally an infinitive compound with _qma_ -make makes words transitive. > @@ -1837,7 +1854,7 @@ Only a few words like ``_fer_ -carry'' are intrinsically transitive. Normally an [you1 (qo se fo) is it desire to2 [to1 [[(qo se fo2) food1/eat/ by to's2 [push boat1 and2 carry2] device1] and [(device1) tear2 (qo se fo2)] complete] < speaker/cry/ (eye1) \desire2\> < experiencer/attention/ (qo se fo1) \desire2\> < time/present/ (something2) \desire1\>]] -A complicated sentence, just as efficient as English. See how Josepho is replicated into the internal infinitives. ``Push-device'' is used for ``propeller''; ``completely eat-tear'' is ``chew up''. The speaker and time are added automatically. +A complicated sentence, just as efficient as English. See how Josepho is replicated into the internal infinitives. Push-device is used for propeller; completely eat-tear is chew up. The speaker and time are added automatically. > @@ -1865,7 +1882,7 @@ A complicated sentence, just as efficient as English. See how Josepho is replica [I1 (eye) must to2 [(eye1) watch2 this2 < time/present/ all something2 \watch1\>] < performer/think/ variable b1 (eye) \must2\> < listener/think/ (eye3) \must2\> < actor/angry/ (eye2) I1 (eye) \must3\> < time/present/ (something2) \must1\>] -A subordinate clause gives, literally, ``at all times''. +A subordinate clause gives, literally, at all times. > @@ -1921,7 +1938,7 @@ For a reflexive, repeat the argument. [I1 (eye) swim < duration all* one's2 second1 \swim2\> < performer/think/ variable b1 (eye) \swim2\> < listener/think/ (eye3) (swim2)> < time/present/ (something2) \swim1\>] -Here is another tense, this time a continuous one. ``One second'' is a simple mathematical expression. +Here is another tense, this time a continuous one. One second is a simple mathematical expression. > @@ -1949,9 +1966,9 @@ Here is another tense, this time a continuous one. ``One second'' is a simple ma [conjunction \swim1\ sufficient to2 [to2 [(this2) trouble] change by this1] < performer/think/ variable b1 (eye) (sufficient2)> < listener/think/ (eye3) (sufficient2)> < time/present/ (something2) \sufficient1\>] -_Gua\spi_ is much richer in causal connectives than English with its ambiguous ``because''; thus this _gua\spi_ sentence is half the length of its English translation. +_Gua\spi_ is much richer in causal connectives than English with its ambiguous because; thus this _gua\spi_ sentence is half the length of its English translation. -This short sample exercises almost every feature of _gua\spi_ \, even including a mathematical expression. To verify every phrase the reader must know _gua\spi_ fairly well, but one can see easily the simple phrase organization of _gua\spi_. The lengths of the _gua\spi_ and native English sentences are comparable, showing how efficient _gua\spi_ is. The primitive words cover almost all meanings in this relatively unspecialized text, and the componds for ``barge'' and ``propeller'' are quite understandable. Finally, the sample makes it clear that _gua\spi_ is more than just a dry substitute for SQL; _gua\spi_ can support real life. +This short sample exercises almost every feature of _gua\spi_ \, even including a mathematical expression. To verify every phrase the reader must know _gua\spi_ fairly well, but one can see easily the simple phrase organization of _gua\spi_. The lengths of the _gua\spi_ and native English sentences are comparable, showing how efficient _gua\spi_ is. The primitive words cover almost all meanings in this relatively unspecialized text, and the componds for barge and propeller are quite understandable. Finally, the sample makes it clear that _gua\spi_ is more than just a dry substitute for SQL; _gua\spi_ can support real life. ## Conclusion @@ -1971,7 +1988,7 @@ I hope this brief introduction to _gua\spi_ has whetted your appetite to learn m ## Appendix: _Gua!spi_ Grammar in Backus-Naur Form -``Discourse'' is the root grameme. Grammar for quoted non-_gua\spi_ text is not shown, but foreign predicates and quoted _gua\spi_ are processed by this grammar and are recognized at the organizational syntax level. A procedural definition shows the simplicity of the grammar more clearly. +Discourse is the root grameme. Grammar for quoted non-_gua\spi_ text is not shown, but foreign predicates and quoted _gua\spi_ are processed by this grammar and are recognized at the organizational syntax level. A procedural definition shows the simplicity of the grammar more clearly. @@ -2073,11 +2090,15 @@ I hope this brief introduction to _gua\spi_ has whetted your appetite to learn m @@ -2089,11 +2110,15 @@ I hope this brief introduction to _gua\spi_ has whetted your appetite to learn m @@ -2213,7 +2238,9 @@ I hope this brief introduction to _gua\spi_ has whetted your appetite to learn m @@ -2291,7 +2318,9 @@ I hope this brief introduction to _gua\spi_ has whetted your appetite to learn m @@ -2356,7 +2385,7 @@ I hope this brief introduction to _gua\spi_ has whetted your appetite to learn m * [NB2] Brown, James C. A Proposed Revision in the Structure of Loglan Words (Notebook No. 2). The Loglan Institute, Inc., Gainesville, Fla., 1982. * [TL43] Parks-Clifford, J. Supplement to Loglan 1. \sl The Loglanist 4, 3 (Nov. 1980). * [La] Information about _Loglan_ may be obtained from The Loglan Institute, Inc., 1701 NE 75th Street, Gainesville, FL 32601. - * [Lja] A modernized version of _Loglan_ , much closer to the original _Loglan_ than _gua\spi_ , is _Lojban_. Information is available from The Logical Languages Group, 2904 Beau Lane, Fairfax, VA 22031, or lojbab@lojban.org. For on-line access, send a message whose body is ``index lojban'' to listserv@hebrew.cc.columbia.edu. + * [Lja] A modernized version of _Loglan_ , much closer to the original _Loglan_ than _gua\spi_ , is _Lojban_. Information is available from The Logical Languages Group, 2904 Beau Lane, Fairfax, VA 22031, or lojbab@lojban.org. For on-line access, send a message whose body is index lojban to listserv@hebrew.cc.columbia.edu. * [Ga] Please contact the author at UCLA Department of Mathematics, Los Angeles, CA 90024-1555, or e-mail to jimc@math.ucla.edu. diff --git a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/cases.md b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/cases.md index bc00b4fb5..667ecd184 100644 --- a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/cases.md +++ b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/cases.md @@ -62,11 +62,11 @@ At the beginning we will use this sentence for an example: -Please pronounce it correctly: `c' as English `ch' and `i' as `ee'. Mind the tones, lest you change it into ``the eat rats the cheese'' or some such. Since no dictionary is included with this paper, in examples where it is hard to match up the _gua\spi_ and English words the English translations are augmented with a ``pidgin'' translation using _gua\spi_ word order. The notation ``_tara_ -rat'' in examples means that _``tara''_ is the example word, and it means ``rat'' in English. Isolated words or phrases like this are written without a tone because it depends on the context where the word is used. +Please pronounce it correctly: `c' as English `ch' and `i' as `ee'. Mind the tones, lest you change it into the eat rats the cheese or some such. Since no dictionary is included with this paper, in examples where it is hard to match up the _gua\spi_ and English words the English translations are augmented with a pidgin translation using _gua\spi_ word order. The notation _tara_ -rat in examples means that _tara_ is the example word, and it means rat in English. Isolated words or phrases like this are written without a tone because it depends on the context where the word is used. ## What is a Predicate -Human languages generally distinguish between ``things'' and ``actions'', where an ``action'' is a relation between ``things''. The formal term for such a relation is a ``predicate''. Take for example: +Human languages generally distinguish between things and actions, where an action is a relation between things. The formal term for such a relation is a predicate. Take for example: > @@ -92,11 +92,11 @@ Human languages generally distinguish between ``things'' and ``actions'', where -``_crw_ -eats'', called a ``predicate word'', tells how the rat and the cheese are related and is a symbol for a certain predicate. The predicate is like a function whose arguments are things that might be related; the value of the function is true or false (or fuzzily in between) depending on whether or not they actually are thus related: in this sentence, whether the first actual parameter eats the second. +_crw_ -eats, called a predicate word, tells how the rat and the cheese are related and is a symbol for a certain predicate. The predicate is like a function whose arguments are things that might be related; the value of the function is true or false (or fuzzily in between) depending on whether or not they actually are thus related: in this sentence, whether the first actual parameter eats the second. -The formal parameters of a predicate, regarded as a function, are referred to as ``cases''. English has ``nominative'' and ``accusative'' cases (the rat occupies the nominative case and the cheese occupies the accusative case), and Latin has in addition ``genitive'', ``vocative'' and others, but _gua\spi_ simply numbers the cases. Some _gua\spi_ words have as many as five numbered cases. In our example, ``_tara_ -rat'' fills the first case of ``_crw_ -eats'' and ``_kseo_ -cheese'' fills the second. +The formal parameters of a predicate, regarded as a function, are referred to as cases. English has nominative and accusative cases (the rat occupies the nominative case and the cheese occupies the accusative case), and Latin has in addition genitive, vocative and others, but _gua\spi_ simply numbers the cases. Some _gua\spi_ words have as many as five numbered cases. In our example, _tara_ -rat fills the first case of _crw_ -eats and _kseo_ -cheese fills the second. -The words denoting the actual parameters of a predicate are called ``arguments''; being sub-phrases, they have their own predicate words. Here, _``!tara''_ and _``!kseo''_ are the arguments. The ``thing'' represented by an argument, which is the actual parameter of the sentence predicate, is something that can fill the first case of the argument's predicate. It is referred to as the ``referent'' of the argument. For example, +The words denoting the actual parameters of a predicate are called arguments; being sub-phrases, they have their own predicate words. Here, _!tara_ and _!kseo_ are the arguments. The thing represented by an argument, which is the actual parameter of the sentence predicate, is something that can fill the first case of the argument's predicate. It is referred to as the referent of the argument. For example, > @@ -126,15 +126,15 @@ is an argument phrase; the first case is left open, and our rat (which we have s Not every first case occupant is a referent of the argument. The rules for forming the referent subset are presented later. -A predicate might have only one case. Some such words are actions in English, like ``_vde_ -alive'', but most are things, like ``_tara_ -rat''. By itself ``_tara_ -rat'' is an argument, and its open first case can be filled by any one of numerous rats. +A predicate might have only one case. Some such words are actions in English, like _vde_ -alive, but most are things, like _tara_ -rat. By itself _tara_ -rat is an argument, and its open first case can be filled by any one of numerous rats. -In literate English and most other languages a word should not be both a noun and a verb, but in _gua\spi_ any predicate word can play either role depending on cues recognized at the organizational syntax level; the grammar is the same for sentences and arguments. This unification cuts in half the complexity of the language, which is already simple. The term ``phrase'' will be used to mean either a sentence or an argument. +In literate English and most other languages a word should not be both a noun and a verb, but in _gua\spi_ any predicate word can play either role depending on cues recognized at the organizational syntax level; the grammar is the same for sentences and arguments. This unification cuts in half the complexity of the language, which is already simple. The term phrase will be used to mean either a sentence or an argument. ## What Definitions Mean -A predicate word expresses a relation between the occupants of its cases. In English and all natural languages, words are ``defined'' by a sentence or two; the words in those sentences are often defined circularly in terms of the word being defined. In _gua\spi_ , on the other hand, the text definition is merely a learning aid. The relation is actually defined by a set of all thus-related object lists. For example, the referent set of ``eats'' includes a member with our example rat in first case and our example cheese in second, as well as numerous other members containing rats, foods, and so on ad (almost literally) infinitum. Other predicates (such as ``pair'') have referent sets that are actually infinite. +A predicate word expresses a relation between the occupants of its cases. In English and all natural languages, words are defined by a sentence or two; the words in those sentences are often defined circularly in terms of the word being defined. In _gua\spi_ , on the other hand, the text definition is merely a learning aid. The relation is actually defined by a set of all thus-related object lists. For example, the referent set of eats includes a member with our example rat in first case and our example cheese in second, as well as numerous other members containing rats, foods, and so on ad (almost literally) infinitum. Other predicates (such as pair) have referent sets that are actually infinite. -Language users are not expected to be familiar with every object set that was, is now or ever shall be thus related. A big part of language behavior consists of the listener adding to his knowledge of which items are thus related, which information the speaker sends to him. Each person has his own limited experience of the world, but we speak of ``the referent set'' of a word independent of a person because words are supposed to mean the same thing to each person, that is, if a person is aware of a particular referent set member, typically he will agree with other language users which word's definition it is a member of. +Language users are not expected to be familiar with every object set that was, is now or ever shall be thus related. A big part of language behavior consists of the listener adding to his knowledge of which items are thus related, which information the speaker sends to him. Each person has his own limited experience of the world, but we speak of the referent set of a word independent of a person because words are supposed to mean the same thing to each person, that is, if a person is aware of a particular referent set member, typically he will agree with other language users which word's definition it is a member of. Humans are very good at generalizing from a few referent set members so as to recognize novel referents, and they are not satisfied with a word until they can do such a general recognition algorithm and usually come out with the same answers their neighbors do. But mechanical users of _gua\spi_ cannot be expected to show such skill, and neither can beginning human users such as infants. They must build up a referent set for a word by exhaustively hearing referent set members. If an advanced human, or advanced software, can transcend the official definition of _gua\spi_ words, that's fine --- a common (but risky) strategy for humans will be to use their native language as a guide to _gua\spi_ meanings. However, _gua\spi_ words are still defined officially in terms of referent sets simply because this definition is known to be tractable both for theory and for practical implementation. A _gua\spi_ referent set is perfectly suited to be represented as a Prolog database, if truncated to a practical size. @@ -192,7 +192,7 @@ When you speak a sentence or a subordinate assertion you do the same thing: you -your knowledge of the referent set of ``_jun_ -hunt'' includes a member which John will want to append to the ones he knows, before the cheese is stolen. This is the ultimate meaning of the _gua\spi_ sentence. +your knowledge of the referent set of _jun_ -hunt includes a member which John will want to append to the ones he knows, before the cheese is stolen. This is the ultimate meaning of the _gua\spi_ sentence. # Organization @@ -200,11 +200,11 @@ Now that we have an unambiguous parse tree made up of phrases, what shall we do ## Which Words Go in Which Cases -The tones of grammar deliver to the organizational syntax level, for each phrase, an ordered list of attached sub-phrases, which are the arguments of the phrase predicate. For example in _``!tara /crw !kseo''_ , ``_tara_ -rat'' and ``_kseo_ -cheese'' are attached to ``_crw_ -eats'' as sub-phrases and therefore are its arguments. In the simplest and most common variation the arguments fill a sentence predicate's cases in order by number, much like English and Chinese, so ``_tara_ -rat'' fills the first case of ``_crw_ -eats'' and ``_kseo_ -cheese'' fills the second. In arguments the first case is left unfilled. This organizational syntax can be so simple because the grammar delivers unambiguous lists of arguments, whereas in English or Latin a combined syntax has to deal with both getting the arguments on the right predicates and getting them into the right cases, and so is a lot more complicated. +The tones of grammar deliver to the organizational syntax level, for each phrase, an ordered list of attached sub-phrases, which are the arguments of the phrase predicate. For example in _!tara /crw !kseo_ , _tara_ -rat and _kseo_ -cheese are attached to _crw_ -eats as sub-phrases and therefore are its arguments. In the simplest and most common variation the arguments fill a sentence predicate's cases in order by number, much like English and Chinese, so _tara_ -rat fills the first case of _crw_ -eats and _kseo_ -cheese fills the second. In arguments the first case is left unfilled. This organizational syntax can be so simple because the grammar delivers unambiguous lists of arguments, whereas in English or Latin a combined syntax has to deal with both getting the arguments on the right predicates and getting them into the right cases, and so is a lot more complicated. The root phrase is assumed, in the absence of special cue words, to be a sentence; thus its first sub-phrase fills its first case. All sub-phrases are assumed to be arguments with empty first cases, except if they have tones or prefixed cue words that make them subordinate or infinitive clauses. -Should it be inconvenient to have cases filled in order, _gua\spi_ has ways to change the order. First, certain prefixes signify that the relation word is ``converted'': a certain case is exchanged with the first and so brought to the front. This is most useful for arguments. For example in _``zu -crw''_ the first and second cases are exchanged, and the referent of such an argument would be something occupying the second case of ``eats'' before conversion: the meaning is ``food''. The second case after conversion would then be the eater: _``zu -crw !xo -tara''_ means ``rat food''. The most common converted meanings have words of their own, such as ``_kqu_ -food''. Here is a florid example of conversion, in which one word serves for a sentence predicate and five different argument predicates: +Should it be inconvenient to have cases filled in order, _gua\spi_ has ways to change the order. First, certain prefixes signify that the relation word is converted: a certain case is exchanged with the first and so brought to the front. This is most useful for arguments. For example in _zu -crw_ the first and second cases are exchanged, and the referent of such an argument would be something occupying the second case of eats before conversion: the meaning is food. The second case after conversion would then be the eater: _zu -crw !xo -tara_ means rat food. The most common converted meanings have words of their own, such as _kqu_ -food. Here is a florid example of conversion, in which one word serves for a sentence predicate and five different argument predicates: > @@ -299,7 +299,7 @@ Should it be inconvenient to have cases filled in order, _gua\spi_ has ways to c Definitions show the case numbers as X1, X2, etc. A caselink or a phrase-relative pronoun (described later) that pertains to a particular case finds that case wherever in the argument list it has been moved by conversion. Similarly, if there are several conversions on one predicate (not recommended) the one closest to the predicate has effect first, and the next one exchanges some case, wherever moved, with the new first sequential case. -Second, an argument can be directed to a specific case by a ``caselink'' prefix. For example, take +Second, an argument can be directed to a specific case by a caselink prefix. For example, take > @@ -326,19 +326,19 @@ Second, an argument can be directed to a specific case by a ``caselink'' prefix. -_``qo -jan''_ is ``John''; _``qo''_ marks a foreign name. _``fer''_ = ``X1 carries X2 to X3 from X4 via X5''. Its arguments are _``qo -jan''_ in the first case, but _``se''_ links the next argument, ``_dowu_ -house'', to the fourth case: the start point. The caselink _``se''_ attaches to and is one level down from the sentence predicate _``fer''_ , hence would have falling tone. The argument predicate _``dowu''_ attaches to _``se''_ as a compound, and hence has high even tone. Sequential arguments jump over cases filled by caselinks. +_qo -jan_ is John; _qo_ marks a foreign name. _fer_ = X1 carries X2 to X3 from X4 via X5. Its arguments are _qo -jan_ in the first case, but _se_ links the next argument, _dowu_ -house, to the fourth case: the start point. The caselink _se_ attaches to and is one level down from the sentence predicate _fer_ , hence would have falling tone. The argument predicate _dowu_ attaches to _se_ as a compound, and hence has high even tone. Sequential arguments jump over cases filled by caselinks. -English and many other natural languages use a ``subject-verb-object'' word order with the actor first, but in _gua\spi_ the predicate can occur before, after or among the arguments. A sentence start word, or in sub-phrases some other prefix word at the same level as the predicate, will always occur before all of the arguments and will provide a jump point from which their grammatical levels can be established. +English and many other natural languages use a subject-verb-object word order with the actor first, but in _gua\spi_ the predicate can occur before, after or among the arguments. A sentence start word, or in sub-phrases some other prefix word at the same level as the predicate, will always occur before all of the arguments and will provide a jump point from which their grammatical levels can be established. Since listeners like subject-verb-object order you should use it when possible, but listeners also like to hear complicated phrases at the end of a sentence, and you can achieve this goal by judiciously moving the predicate, by converting it, or by delaying a complicated argument to the end of the sentence and using an explicit caselink word. In English, converting the predicate produces the ``passive voice'', which has a somewhat different meaning than the standard word order. No such passivity attaches to a converted _gua\spi_ predicate. It is a fact, though, that listeners like the actor to be first when it can be expressed in one or two words, and do not like it to be omitted --- common mistakes when people use the English passive voice. -It is permitted to say one or more arguments in isolation. This construction is called a ``nonsentence''. It begins with the usual sentence start word _``:i''_ and the arguments, as usual, are one level down, but there just isn't any sentence predicate. +It is permitted to say one or more arguments in isolation. This construction is called a nonsentence. It begins with the usual sentence start word _:i_ and the arguments, as usual, are one level down, but there just isn't any sentence predicate. ## Sentences as Arguments --- Infinitives -A _gua\spi_ sentence or argument expresses a relation between specific referents, and this specific referent set member is called an ``event''. (Frequently the sentence represents several similar events.) It is common for several cases of the predicate to be vacant: in the previous example the thing carried, the destination and the route were not specified. Nonetheless there must have been a thing carried, a destination and a route, and the sentence asserts a relation between all five arguments. The next organizational elements we will look at are linking words that attach sentence predicates (with their arguments). The linked sentences represent lists of specific events with specific argument referents and with all cases filled even if not specified by words. +A _gua\spi_ sentence or argument expresses a relation between specific referents, and this specific referent set member is called an event. (Frequently the sentence represents several similar events.) It is common for several cases of the predicate to be vacant: in the previous example the thing carried, the destination and the route were not specified. Nonetheless there must have been a thing carried, a destination and a route, and the sentence asserts a relation between all five arguments. The next organizational elements we will look at are linking words that attach sentence predicates (with their arguments). The linked sentences represent lists of specific events with specific argument referents and with all cases filled even if not specified by words. -Returning to organization, the first sentence link word is _``vo''_ , which acts to convert a sentence into a one-argument predicate, referred to as an ``infinitive'', which means that the occupant of its first case is an instance of the sentence relation. Though _``vo''_ can itself be a sentence predicate it is much more commonly used in arguments, like this: +Returning to organization, the first sentence link word is _vo_ , which acts to convert a sentence into a one-argument predicate, referred to as an infinitive, which means that the occupant of its first case is an instance of the sentence relation. Though _vo_ can itself be a sentence predicate it is much more commonly used in arguments, like this: > @@ -364,9 +364,9 @@ Returning to organization, the first sentence link word is _``vo''_ , which acts -_``vyu''_ means ``X1 enjoys doing (_vo_) X2'', where the second case is some kind of activity --- a natural place to fill with an infinitive. The sentence linked by _``vo''_ is _``qia !ji''_ = ``I bathe'', and an instance of that relation, an event, is the referent of the argument _``vo -qia !ji''_ = ``my bath''. +_vyu_ means X1 enjoys doing (_vo_) X2, where the second case is some kind of activity --- a natural place to fill with an infinitive. The sentence linked by _vo_ is _qia !ji_ = ``I bathe'', and an instance of that relation, an event, is the referent of the argument _vo -qia !ji_ = my bath. -_``vyu''_ includes the prefix _``vo''_ on its second case by default, as do all words which commonly have infinitive arguments. Also, such words have various patterns, specified in the dictionary, in which main sentence arguments are replicated into infinitives. The most common is for the argument just before the infinitive to be replicated into the infinitive's first case, if the infinitive has no argument caselinked into the first case with _``so''_. Here _``!ji''_ is replicated. So you could say +_vyu_ includes the prefix _vo_ on its second case by default, as do all words which commonly have infinitive arguments. Also, such words have various patterns, specified in the dictionary, in which main sentence arguments are replicated into infinitives. The most common is for the argument just before the infinitive to be replicated into the infinitive's first case, if the infinitive has no argument caselinked into the first case with _so_. Here _!ji_ is replicated. So you could say > @@ -396,10 +396,10 @@ The extensive defaults on structure words, of which the default _vo_ is one of t ## Subordinate Clauses -A subordinate clause, indicated by the linking prefix _``vu''_ , is a sentence within a sentence. Its most common use is to restrict a phrase (an argument or a sentence), so that a thing can be a referent of an argument only if it actually fits in the subordinate sentence, or the main sentence represents only events that fit in the subordinate sentence. Subordinate clauses are more common in _gua\spi_ than in English, and also can be complicated, so several special rules are provided to make them simpler: +A subordinate clause, indicated by the linking prefix _vu_ , is a sentence within a sentence. Its most common use is to restrict a phrase (an argument or a sentence), so that a thing can be a referent of an argument only if it actually fits in the subordinate sentence, or the main sentence represents only events that fit in the subordinate sentence. Subordinate clauses are more common in _gua\spi_ than in English, and also can be complicated, so several special rules are provided to make them simpler: - * Because subordinate clauses are so common the tone `|' is allocated specifically to them which automatically supplies the linking prefix _``vu''_. When this tone does not apply, of course, _``vu''_ may be used explicitly. - * When the predicate of a subordinate clause has a case for an event, indicated by default _``vo''_ or _``bi''_ , the predicate is automatically converted to put the event first. + * Because subordinate clauses are so common the tone `|' is allocated specifically to them which automatically supplies the linking prefix _vu_. When this tone does not apply, of course, _vu_ may be used explicitly. + * When the predicate of a subordinate clause has a case for an event, indicated by default _vo_ or _bi_ , the predicate is automatically converted to put the event first. * The restricted phrase is automatically replicated in the first case of the clause which, if the previous rule applies, will be the event argument. @@ -439,7 +439,7 @@ Here is a subordinate clause restricting an argument: -Not all apples (_ftu -plyw_) but only those which are green (_xgi_) are described as being sour (_qke_). The restricting sentence is ``X1 is green'', and argument referents (apples) are automatically plugged into X1. The second version of the sentence is re-ordered to sound better; subordinate clauses usually do better near the end of the sentence. When in English we use adjectives and adverbs, in _gua\spi_ we usually use subordinate clauses like this one. +Not all apples (_ftu -plyw_) but only those which are green (_xgi_) are described as being sour (_qke_). The restricting sentence is X1 is green, and argument referents (apples) are automatically plugged into X1. The second version of the sentence is re-ordered to sound better; subordinate clauses usually do better near the end of the sentence. When in English we use adjectives and adverbs, in _gua\spi_ we usually use subordinate clauses like this one. Here is a subordinate clause restricting a sentence: @@ -468,11 +468,11 @@ Here is a subordinate clause restricting a sentence: -The restricting sentence is _``!vo -X1 /zu -tum !vden !xdro''_ = ``[X1 is done] with its teeth as a tool'', and the asserted relation _``!tara /cie -pne !kara''_ = ``The rat penetrates the box'' is also required to satisfy the subordinate clause. The effect is as if an additional case were added to ``_cie_ -cut'' for the cutting tool. Note that _``tum''_ auto-converts so that _``zu''_ is not needed in the subordinate clause. +The restricting sentence is _!vo -X1 /zu -tum !vden !xdro_ = [X1 is done] with its teeth as a tool, and the asserted relation _!tara /cie -pne !kara_ = The rat penetrates the box is also required to satisfy the subordinate clause. The effect is as if an additional case were added to _cie_ -cut for the cutting tool. Note that _tum_ auto-converts so that _zu_ is not needed in the subordinate clause. -The additional cases produced by subordinate clauses like this are called ``modal cases''. They specify tenses, locations, listeners (vocative case), speakers in dialogue, repeated actions, and numerous miscellaneous cases as in the previous sentence. These cases are the ``context'' of a sentence. _Gua\spi_ handles the context in a well-defined manner, whereas other languages handle context informally. As with numbered cases, something must fill every modal case in each event even if no words specify what that thing is. For example, many events are done ``by means of'' something, though rarely do we put words to the instrument. Many predicates in the language can give rise to modal cases. Therefore a predicate potentially can have a near-infinite number of cases. +The additional cases produced by subordinate clauses like this are called modal cases. They specify tenses, locations, listeners (vocative case), speakers in dialogue, repeated actions, and numerous miscellaneous cases as in the previous sentence. These cases are the context of a sentence. _Gua\spi_ handles the context in a well-defined manner, whereas other languages handle context informally. As with numbered cases, something must fill every modal case in each event even if no words specify what that thing is. For example, many events are done by means of something, though rarely do we put words to the instrument. Many predicates in the language can give rise to modal cases. Therefore a predicate potentially can have a near-infinite number of cases. -_Gua\spi_ has two other clause link words: _``va''_ for subordinate assertions and _``vi''_ for decorations that show the relation between sentences and the speaker's attitude about a sentence. Their syntax is the same as _``vu''_. For example (_go_ being a mood marker for negation), +_Gua\spi_ has two other clause link words: _va_ for subordinate assertions and _vi_ for decorations that show the relation between sentences and the speaker's attitude about a sentence. Their syntax is the same as _vu_. For example (_go_ being a mood marker for negation), > @@ -509,7 +509,7 @@ _Gua\spi_ has two other clause link words: _``va''_ for subordinate assertions a -In ``_tan_ -annoy'' of the second sentence, who is annoyed? ``_ji_ -me'' is provided by default in the first case (before conversion) of any subordinate clause or top-level sentence whose first case ends up vacant, like this one. Thus top-level exclamations also become more natural: +In _tan_ -annoy of the second sentence, who is annoyed? _ji_ -me is provided by default in the first case (before conversion) of any subordinate clause or top-level sentence whose first case ends up vacant, like this one. Thus top-level exclamations also become more natural: > @@ -574,7 +574,9 @@ A special argument form is quoted text. A quote is a prefix that transforms the @@ -605,7 +607,9 @@ A special argument form is quoted text. A quote is a prefix that transforms the @@ -685,7 +689,9 @@ A special argument form is quoted text. A quote is a prefix that transforms the , up to endmark - ``ba'' + + ba + @@ -703,7 +709,9 @@ A special argument form is quoted text. A quote is a prefix that transforms the @@ -734,7 +742,9 @@ A special argument form is quoted text. A quote is a prefix that transforms the @@ -744,9 +754,9 @@ A special argument form is quoted text. A quote is a prefix that transforms the -The last example, the ``slash string'' quote _``br''_ , is the same as _``ba''_ except that an arbitrary word (_``xa''_ in the example) comes before and after the quoted text, in case _``be''_ cannot be the endmark because it occurs in the text. _``ba''_ and _``br''_ are actually interpreted as part of the grammar, as very special cases, while the rest are recognized at the organizational level. +The last example, the slash string quote _br_ , is the same as _ba_ except that an arbitrary word (_xa_ in the example) comes before and after the quoted text, in case _be_ cannot be the endmark because it occurs in the text. _ba_ and _br_ are actually interpreted as part of the grammar, as very special cases, while the rest are recognized at the organizational level. -Story dialog is represented by quoted sub-phrases in English, but in _gua\spi_ the dialog is at the main level and the speaker and listener are identified by a modal case with ``_jai_ -say'', ``_kam_ -cry'' and related words. These words are defined as ``X1 says text (_bi_) X2 to X3'', with _``bi''_ as the default prefix. Since _``bi''_ is an infinitive prefix, _``jai''_ automatically converts in a modal phrase so the sentence is first. For example, +Story dialog is represented by quoted sub-phrases in English, but in _gua\spi_ the dialog is at the main level and the speaker and listener are identified by a modal case with _jai_ -say, _kam_ -cry and related words. These words are defined as X1 says text (_bi_) X2 to X3, with _bi_ as the default prefix. Since _bi_ is an infinitive prefix, _jai_ automatically converts in a modal phrase so the sentence is first. For example, > @@ -764,7 +774,10 @@ Story dialog is represented by quoted sub-phrases in English, but in _gua\spi_ t diff --git a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/conclusn.md b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/conclusn.md index cc2fbec14..59f79c5a4 100644 --- a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/conclusn.md +++ b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/conclusn.md @@ -137,11 +137,15 @@ For computer applications the normal way to represent _gua\spi_ syntax is in a s @@ -153,11 +157,15 @@ For computer applications the normal way to represent _gua\spi_ syntax is in a s @@ -277,7 +285,9 @@ For computer applications the normal way to represent _gua\spi_ syntax is in a s @@ -355,7 +365,9 @@ For computer applications the normal way to represent _gua\spi_ syntax is in a s @@ -413,18 +425,18 @@ For computer applications the normal way to represent _gua\spi_ syntax is in a s -``Discourse'' is the root grameme. Grammar for quoted non-_gua\spi_ text is not shown, but foreign predicates and quoted _gua\spi_ are processed by this grammar and are put together at the organizational syntax level. +Discourse is the root grameme. Grammar for quoted non-_gua\spi_ text is not shown, but foreign predicates and quoted _gua\spi_ are processed by this grammar and are put together at the organizational syntax level. ## Organizational Transformations Formal syntax is finished at this point, and transformation begins, in this sequence of steps: - * Transform the tone `|' into ``_!vu_ -subordinate clause''. + * Transform the tone `|' into _!vu_ -subordinate clause. * Do the transformations for retroactive downjumps and for error correction (_fa_). - * Distinguish arguments from sentences. Insert _``!so -jy''_ as the placeholder for arguments' open first cases. + * Distinguish arguments from sentences. Insert _!so -jy_ as the placeholder for arguments' open first cases. * Re-order argument lists according to caselinks and conversions. Insert placeholders for missing cases. - * Look up each word in the dictionary. Insert default articles, typically ``_xe_ -the'', before arguments. Insert default _``vo''_. - * In cases of compounding, replicate argument lists for parallel arguments; insert _``vo''_ for compound infinitives; or demote a compound object into the argument list. + * Look up each word in the dictionary. Insert default articles, typically _xe_ -the, before arguments. Insert default _vo_. + * In cases of compounding, replicate argument lists for parallel arguments; insert _vo_ for compound infinitives; or demote a compound object into the argument list. * Replicate main phrase arguments into infinitives. * Substitute the antecedents for phrase-relative, modal and question pronouns. The antecedent of a question pronoun is found in the future answer. * Deal with modal case stack operations. @@ -447,7 +459,7 @@ I hope this brief introduction to _gua\spi_ has whetted your appetite to learn m * [NB2] Brown, James C. A Proposed Revision in the Structure of Loglan Words (Notebook No. 2). The Loglan Institute, Inc., Gainesville, Fla., 1982. * [TL43] Parks-Clifford, J. Supplement to Loglan 1. \sl The Loglanist 4, 3 (Nov. 1980). * [La] Information about _Loglan_ may be obtained from The Loglan Institute, Inc., 1701 NE 75th Street, Gainesville, FL 32601. - * [Lja] A modernized version of _Loglan_ , much closer to the original _Loglan_ than _gua\spi_ , is _Lojban_. Information is available from The Logical Languages Group, 2904 Beau Lane, Fairfax, VA 22031, or lojbab@lojban.org. For on-line access, send a message whose body is ``index lojban'' to listserv@hebrew.cc.columbia.edu. + * [Lja] A modernized version of _Loglan_ , much closer to the original _Loglan_ than _gua\spi_ , is _Lojban_. Information is available from The Logical Languages Group, 2904 Beau Lane, Fairfax, VA 22031, or lojbab@lojban.org. For on-line access, send a message whose body is index lojban to listserv@hebrew.cc.columbia.edu. * [Ga] Please contact the author at UCLA Department of Mathematics, Los Angeles, CA 90024-1555, or e-mail to jimc@math.ucla.edu. diff --git a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/dictintr.md b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/dictintr.md index 4d4912309..177bb3a58 100644 --- a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/dictintr.md +++ b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/dictintr.md @@ -10,7 +10,7 @@ This is the dictionary of the _gua\spi_ language. It is organized into three parts in which words are ordered by _gua\spi_ spelling (with morphological formats in separate sections), by the English translation, and by thesaurus categories. The table of contents also serves as an outline of the categories. -This edition includes only primitive words of _gua\spi_. Only a few compound words appear. A future project will be to go through a list of the N most common English words and either to verify that each one has a primitive translation or to recommend a compound representation of it. Note the word ``recommend''. The speaker of _gua\spi_ is expected to represent his meaning by compounding primitive words creatively, and the architect of _gua\spi_ does not insist on particular compound words to translate particular English words. +This edition includes only primitive words of _gua\spi_. Only a few compound words appear. A future project will be to go through a list of the N most common English words and either to verify that each one has a primitive translation or to recommend a compound representation of it. Note the word recommend. The speaker of _gua\spi_ is expected to represent his meaning by compounding primitive words creatively, and the architect of _gua\spi_ does not insist on particular compound words to translate particular English words. But since the vocabulary of English is so vast, even the working vocabulary of an educated person, most English words will never be in this dictionary. Here is where the thesaurus can help. Look at the primitive words in the category where your meaning is, and try to modify one of them by compounding to achieve the meaning you want. @@ -18,20 +18,20 @@ To get the most use out of _gua\spi_ you have to know all the primitive words. F Be sure to remember that _gua\spi_ predicates can be converted. The English word used in the definition is the one most representative of the unconverted _gua\spi_ case order, but by conversion you can get up to four additional English meanings. -In one trial of _Loglan_ about half of the predicates were compound, and likely more will be compound in _gua\spi_. Be alert for creative opportunities for expression --- don't use only the words identified as being ``common in compounds''. But resist the temptation to specify a predicate over-precisely with many compounded words; one thinks differently in _gua\spi_ , and mashing _gua\spi_ words to fit English preconceptions does not give the best results. +In one trial of _Loglan_ about half of the predicates were compound, and likely more will be compound in _gua\spi_. Be alert for creative opportunities for expression --- don't use only the words identified as being common in compounds. But resist the temptation to specify a predicate over-precisely with many compounded words; one thinks differently in _gua\spi_ , and mashing _gua\spi_ words to fit English preconceptions does not give the best results. Acronyms, so popular in illiterate English, are useless in _gua\spi_ because the letterals of the acronym are longer than the compound word or phrase that they abbreviate. Again, keep the compound short. -An example of a definition is ``X1 likes to do (vo) X2+1''. The symbols X1 and X2 represent the first and second numbered cases of the predicate ``like''. Words in parentheses before the case symbols, such as ``(vo)'', are provided by default before that argument unless there is an explicit article there --- which cancels both the default article and the default phrase linker if any. All arguments receive _``xe''_ as a prefixed article by default, unless a different article is shown in the dictionary or is said explicitly. Signs following the case numbers indicate special features of compound words and case merging in infinitives. Here they are illustrated decorating the second case, but the same pattern applies for all cases. +An example of a definition is X1 likes to do o X2+1. The symbols X1 and X2 represent the first and second numbered cases of the predicate like. Words in parentheses before the case symbols, such as o, are provided by default before that argument unless there is an explicit article there --- which cancels both the default article and the default phrase linker if any. All arguments receive _xe_ as a prefixed article by default, unless a different article is shown in the dictionary or is said explicitly. Signs following the case numbers indicate special features of compound words and case merging in infinitives. Here they are illustrated decorating the second case, but the same pattern applies for all cases. X2* In a transitive compound (tone `='), this is the case where the object goes. It is the second case by default (but the dictionary even so shows many X2*'s explicitly). X2+1 - In an infinitive compound (tone `-'), this is the case where the infinitive goes. Note that a few words do not do infinitive compounds (they do parallel compounds instead) even though they have a case for an infinitive. All cases of these words lack `+' or have `@'. The `1' selects the first case of the main predicate. This argument is replicated as the first case of the infinitive (whether explicit or from a compound). The notation ``+1,2'' means that referents from both cases are replicated. A `+' alone signals infinitive compounding but with no case replication. + In an infinitive compound (tone `-'), this is the case where the infinitive goes. Note that a few words do not do infinitive compounds (they do parallel compounds instead) even though they have a case for an infinitive. All cases of these words lack `+' or have `@'. The `1' selects the first case of the main predicate. This argument is replicated as the first case of the infinitive (whether explicit or from a compound). The notation +1,2 means that referents from both cases are replicated. A `+' alone signals infinitive compounding but with no case replication. X2-3 Usually used with a `+' decoration, the -3 means that the third case of the main predicate is replicated as the second case of the infinitive. X2+S1 - The `S' means that, X1 normally being a set, its members are replicated in extension into the first case of the infinitive. ``-S1'' is also used for the infinitive second case. + The `S' means that, X1 normally being a set, its members are replicated in extension into the first case of the infinitive. -S1 is also used for the infinitive second case. X2+P1 The `P' means that the infinitive is applied pairwise to members of the first case set, as in sorting or finding an extreme member. X2+1@ @@ -46,7 +46,7 @@ Here are the types of compounds. See the reference manual for a complete explana Infinitive The second word is the predicate of an infinitive in one of the first word's cases. (Tone `-'.) Parallel - Both words share the same arguments so both relations are simultaneously true of each argument list. (Tone `-', or _``-fe''_ if an infinitive compound would supervene.) + Both words share the same arguments so both relations are simultaneously true of each argument list. (Tone `-', or _-fe_ if an infinitive compound would supervene.) Transitive The second word is the predicate of an argument in one of the first word's cases, the second case unless otherwise noted. (Tone `='.) diff --git a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/grammar.md b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/grammar.md index bc2a79079..4503475a8 100644 --- a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/grammar.md +++ b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/grammar.md @@ -75,7 +75,7 @@ Organization Semantics Giving meaning to syntactic structures. -Natural language syntax is extremely complicated because the syntax expresses actual meanings such as tenses and numbers. In _gua\spi_ the first three levels are independent of the meaning of the words. This makes them less interesting than jewels like the ``perfective aspect'' of Russian or the ``long object case'' of Navajo, but it makes them much simpler and much easier to learn and use. +Natural language syntax is extremely complicated because the syntax expresses actual meanings such as tenses and numbers. In _gua\spi_ the first three levels are independent of the meaning of the words. This makes them less interesting than jewels like the perfective aspect of Russian or the long object case of Navajo, but it makes them much simpler and much easier to learn and use. # Morphology: What is a Word @@ -455,13 +455,13 @@ Phonemes can be distinguished by where the tongue is placed to make them, whethe -> Table 2 [Pronunciation]. How to pronounce _gua\spi_ phonemes. Nonstandard C's are shown; C's without examples are as in English. Standard radio broadcast accent is close to correct for the V's; Spanish is closer. Pronounce the vowels as one sound, not a glide between two sounds as in ``eye''. +> Table 2 [Pronunciation]. How to pronounce _gua\spi_ phonemes. Nonstandard C's are shown; C's without examples are as in English. Standard radio broadcast accent is close to correct for the V's; Spanish is closer. Pronounce the vowels as one sound, not a glide between two sounds as in eye. * * * -The sound `#' or `uh' is common in English; all vowel letters are sometimes pronounced `#'. The `a' of ``among'' is a good example. This sound is called ``schwa''; that German name is pronounced (with _gua\spi_ letters) ``sqv#''. `#' is not used in regular words; its purpose is to break up CC pairs that a particular speaker finds hard to pronounce, since virtually all speakers will be able to handle C#C. It is to be ignored and it is only written in explanations like this one. Though normally considered a vowel, it is in the C class because it occurs among C's, and a word is defined as some C's followed by some V's. +The sound `#' or `uh' is common in English; all vowel letters are sometimes pronounced `#'. The `a' of among is a good example. This sound is called schwa; that German name is pronounced (with _gua\spi_ letters) sqv#. `#' is not used in regular words; its purpose is to break up CC pairs that a particular speaker finds hard to pronounce, since virtually all speakers will be able to handle C#C. It is to be ignored and it is only written in explanations like this one. Though normally considered a vowel, it is in the C class because it occurs among C's, and a word is defined as some C's followed by some V's. -The glottal stop `:' pronounced alone is a sudden (plosive) `#', but it is normally followed by a V so that it sounds like a brief pause after which the V comes on. In many English dialects, as in _gua\spi_ , it is found between a vowel-final and vowel-initial word, like ``the:apple'', while the Cockney dialect uses it much more extensively. The glottal stop is not used in regular words; its place is at the beginning of each sentence start word, and in vowel-initial foreign words. +The glottal stop `:' pronounced alone is a sudden (plosive) `#', but it is normally followed by a V so that it sounds like a brief pause after which the V comes on. In many English dialects, as in _gua\spi_ , it is found between a vowel-final and vowel-initial word, like the:apple, while the Cockney dialect uses it much more extensively. The glottal stop is not used in regular words; its place is at the beginning of each sentence start word, and in vowel-initial foreign words. English has thirteen subtly different vowels plus four official diphthongs but only five letters to represent them. _Gua\spi_ uses only six easily distinguished vowel sounds, recruiting Y for one of them, and adds some vowel-like sounds which are considered consonants in English. Unfortunately, many regional accents of English turn simple vowels into diphthongs, invalidating the example words given in [Table 2 [Pronuncation]](grammar.html#Pronunciation). Other accents transform sounds beyond the bounds that a _gua\spi_ speaker can recognize. If you speak with a regional accent, please use the vowel sounds that you can hear on television or radio (American or British will both work). Particularly troublesome examples, rendered with _gua\spi_ letters, are shown in this table: @@ -522,7 +522,11 @@ English has thirteen subtly different vowels plus four official diphthongs but o f%thA @@ -543,7 +547,7 @@ Japanese speakers are famous for producing `l' and `r' that Europeans cannot dis Preliminary experience shows that the errors English-speaking beginners make most often are to interchange `q' with `c', `x' with `j', and `i' with `y'; and to pronounce `w' as `oo' (should be `ng'). -Written blanks have no sound, and are optional. In this document a blank usually comes before each word (except in the phrase ``_gua\spi_ ''), although in running text it looks nicer to omit blanks before the tone `-'. There is no distinction between upper and lower case. The tones (described next) make punctuation unnecessary. There are no periods at the ends of sentences; however, each sentence start word begins with a glottal stop, written as a colon. This colon is a letter, not a punctuation. +Written blanks have no sound, and are optional. In this document a blank usually comes before each word (except in the phrase _gua\spi_), although in running text it looks nicer to omit blanks before the tone `-'. There is no distinction between upper and lower case. The tones (described next) make punctuation unnecessary. There are no periods at the ends of sentences; however, each sentence start word begins with a glottal stop, written as a colon. This colon is a letter, not a punctuation. A feature of _gua\spi_ (like Loglan before it, and unlike English) is that writing and speech are isomorphic, that is, each letter has a single phoneme (sound) and each phoneme has a single letter (with trivial exceptions), so that each spoken text can be spelled easily and without ambiguity, and each written text can be read off equally easily. @@ -553,9 +557,9 @@ The job of grammar is to stick words together into phrases. The grammar does not ## Parse Tree -The grammar is stated in Backus-Naur form in [Section [Backus]](conclusn.html#Backus). For grammatical purposes there is only one kind of phrase (though distinctions are made at the organizational level), but words have five categories: the two words _``fu''_ and _``fi''_ , sentence start words, other prefixes, and everything else. The main part of a phrase is a sequence of one or more words collectively called the ``phrase predicate''; any prefixes in this must come first. After any of the prefixes or after the whole predicate the sub-phrases are interspersed. They, of course, have their own prefixes, predicates and sub-phrases. +The grammar is stated in Backus-Naur form in [Section [Backus]](conclusn.html#Backus). For grammatical purposes there is only one kind of phrase (though distinctions are made at the organizational level), but words have five categories: the two words _fu_ and _fi_ , sentence start words, other prefixes, and everything else. The main part of a phrase is a sequence of one or more words collectively called the phrase predicate; any prefixes in this must come first. After any of the prefixes or after the whole predicate the sub-phrases are interspersed. They, of course, have their own prefixes, predicates and sub-phrases. -Let us understand phrases with the help of the example in the following figure, showing the ``parse tree'' of a simple sentence. The root phrase is at the top; parse trees grow upside down. Sub-phrases with their own predicates come at the next lower level. These in turn may have their own sub-phrases. Each phrase is at a certain level and it attaches to the most recent phrase at the next higher level. The tones (see [Table 4 [Tones]](grammar.html#Tones)) show the level of each word relative to the one before it. +Let us understand phrases with the help of the example in the following figure, showing the parse tree of a simple sentence. The root phrase is at the top; parse trees grow upside down. Sub-phrases with their own predicates come at the next lower level. These in turn may have their own sub-phrases. Each phrase is at a certain level and it attaches to the most recent phrase at the next higher level. The tones (see [Table 4 [Tones]](grammar.html#Tones)) show the level of each word relative to the one before it. * * * @@ -603,13 +607,13 @@ Let us understand phrases with the help of the example in the following figure, * * * -The root phrase predicate of the example sentence is ``devour'' (violently eat), and the start word is at the same level. ``Rat'', attaching to ``violent'', is therefore at the next level below the start word, indicated by `!'. ``Violent'' is at the level above ``rat'', indicated by `/'. ``Eat'' is attached to ``violent'' in a compound, indicated by `-'. For grammatical purposes the pair of words, plus the start word, form the root phrase predicate, although later at the organizational syntax level ``eat'' will be split from ``violent''. Like ``rat'', ``cheese'' attaches below ``eat'' (`!'). The next phrase also attaches to ``eat'', indicated by `^', because ``eat'' is the most recent word at the next level up. It is a subordinate clause, indicated by the prefix _``vu''_ , that tells what was ``used'' to eat; in a similar tone pattern ``fork'' and ``knife'' both attach to ``using''. ``Using'' and ``knife'' attach to their prefixes with compounding tone `-'. To summarize, words attach to the previous word at the next higher level, and the tones of _gua\spi_ represent the attachment level of the present word relative to the one just before it. Every word has such a tone. +The root phrase predicate of the example sentence is devour (violently eat), and the start word is at the same level. Rat, attaching to violent, is therefore at the next level below the start word, indicated by `!'. Violent is at the level above rat, indicated by `/'. Eat is attached to violent in a compound, indicated by `-'. For grammatical purposes the pair of words, plus the start word, form the root phrase predicate, although later at the organizational syntax level eat will be split from violent. Like rat, cheese attaches below eat (`!'). The next phrase also attaches to eat, indicated by `^', because eat is the most recent word at the next level up. It is a subordinate clause, indicated by the prefix _vu_ , that tells what was used to eat; in a similar tone pattern fork and knife both attach to using. Using and knife attach to their prefixes with compounding tone `-'. To summarize, words attach to the previous word at the next higher level, and the tones of _gua\spi_ represent the attachment level of the present word relative to the one just before it. Every word has such a tone. ## The Tones The tones are the most terrifying aspect of _gua\spi_ for speakers of European languages. Please remember that over a billion people in China and Southeast Asia speak tonal languages. If they can do it, so can you. -A tone is a specific change in pitch or vocal frequency. In English a falling tone marks the end of the sentence, a rising tone marks a question, a down-up combination is a kind of verbal comma, and most of the sentence is said at a fairly even tone. Chinese has similar tones but each word has one and the tones distinguish between meanings, e.g. _``-ma''_ (high even tone) means ``mother'' while _``|ma''_ (down-up) means ``horse''. _Gua\spi_ is intermediate in its use of tones: each word has a tone, but rather than affecting the meaning of the word it tells which neighboring word it attaches to. The tones are shown in this table: +A tone is a specific change in pitch or vocal frequency. In English a falling tone marks the end of the sentence, a rising tone marks a question, a down-up combination is a kind of verbal comma, and most of the sentence is said at a fairly even tone. Chinese has similar tones but each word has one and the tones distinguish between meanings, e.g. _-ma_ (high even tone) means mother while _|ma_ (down-up) means horse. _Gua\spi_ is intermediate in its use of tones: each word has a tone, but rather than affecting the meaning of the word it tells which neighboring word it attaches to. The tones are shown in this table: * * * @@ -764,7 +768,7 @@ A tone is a specific change in pitch or vocal frequency. In English a falling to -> Table 4 [Tones]. Sounds and interpretations of the tones. ``Level'' refers to the parse tree level of the word with that tone, relative to the structure before it. ``Type'' indicates the organizational type of that word or phrase. The first set of symbols shown, ascii characters, is preferred but the second set can substitute on a manual typewriter. In this paper, `!' is used instead of `\' for convenience in typesetting. +> Table 4 [Tones]. Sounds and interpretations of the tones. Level refers to the parse tree level of the word with that tone, relative to the structure before it. Type indicates the organizational type of that word or phrase. The first set of symbols shown, ascii characters, is preferred but the second set can substitute on a manual typewriter. In this paper, `!' is used instead of `\' for convenience in typesetting. * * * @@ -772,9 +776,9 @@ You don't speak this language, you sing it. Chinese speakers will have no proble Tones `-' and `=' join adjacent words of a compound phrase predicate. Tones `|' and `!' start a sub-phrase of the current phrase. Tone `^' closes the current sub-phrase and starts a new one, part of the same containing phrase. Tone `/' closes a sub-phrase and resumes the predicate of the containing phrase, if among its prefixes, or otherwise starts a new phrase at the higher level. Distinctions within these tone classes are important later but do not affect the grammar. -A sentence start prefix with up-down tone such as _``^:i''_ is automatically at root level. _``fi''_ jumps to the root level or one or two levels lower without ending the sentence, depending on its tone selected from `!', `^' or `/', as if an imaginary level 1 word preceeded it. If a word is supposed to be more than one level higher (closer to the root) than the previous one but _``fi''_ does not apply, you use _``fu''_ to raise the level count by two to four depending on its tone, selected from `!', `^' or `/' respectively. You can repeat _``fu''_ for even higher levels, but this is rarely necessary since from level 7 or less you can reach any level from 0 (root) to 8 in one jump. On the level-shifting prefixes the tone `-' usually can substitute for `^', except after a foreign word, name or digit, where the prefix tends to get included in the previous structure. +A sentence start prefix with up-down tone such as _^:i_ is automatically at root level. _fi_ jumps to the root level or one or two levels lower without ending the sentence, depending on its tone selected from `!', `^' or `/', as if an imaginary level 1 word preceeded it. If a word is supposed to be more than one level higher (closer to the root) than the previous one but _fi_ does not apply, you use _fu_ to raise the level count by two to four depending on its tone, selected from `!', `^' or `/' respectively. You can repeat _fu_ for even higher levels, but this is rarely necessary since from level 7 or less you can reach any level from 0 (root) to 8 in one jump. On the level-shifting prefixes the tone `-' usually can substitute for `^', except after a foreign word, name or digit, where the prefix tends to get included in the previous structure. -Sentence start words like _``:i''_ and other prefix words like _``se''_ or _``vu''_ come before and at the same level as the predicate of their phrase. The predicate normally has compounding (high even) tone unless some of its arguments precede it, and the linked predicate is considered to be a compound word with the prefix, whether or not arguments come between them. There may be several prefixes in a row before the predicate, all at the same level and linked in sequence. The one closest to the predicate has effect first. +Sentence start words like _:i_ and other prefix words like _se_ or _vu_ come before and at the same level as the predicate of their phrase. The predicate normally has compounding (high even) tone unless some of its arguments precede it, and the linked predicate is considered to be a compound word with the prefix, whether or not arguments come between them. There may be several prefixes in a row before the predicate, all at the same level and linked in sequence. The one closest to the predicate has effect first. As befits a grammatical element, the tone of a word implies what kind of structure is attaching. In particular, the third tone, down-up, `|', is specifically for the very common case of putting a subordinate clause one level down from the previous word. diff --git a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/pronouns.md b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/pronouns.md index 0eba28b67..a4f42d957 100644 --- a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/pronouns.md +++ b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/pronouns.md @@ -33,15 +33,15 @@ ## Pronouns Represent Words, Not Things -The next organizational issue is the pronoun. In English, pronouns have referents just like any other argument. But _gua\spi_ pronouns represent words, not the referent of words. In computer terms, they are like macro-instructions rather than in-line functions. The represented words are called the ``antecedent'' of the pronoun, and the sentence is analysed as if each pronoun were taken out and replaced by its antecedent. The antecedents, not the pronouns, have referents. In this way the organizational syntax level can be kept free of meaning, and the semantic level has to deal with only one class of words, predicates. +The next organizational issue is the pronoun. In English, pronouns have referents just like any other argument. But _gua\spi_ pronouns represent words, not the referent of words. In computer terms, they are like macro-instructions rather than in-line functions. The represented words are called the antecedent of the pronoun, and the sentence is analysed as if each pronoun were taken out and replaced by its antecedent. The antecedents, not the pronouns, have referents. In this way the organizational syntax level can be kept free of meaning, and the semantic level has to deal with only one class of words, predicates. -For example, a document typically will have a signature line saying in effect ``this text is the output of Jim Carter''. (Spoken discourse is completely analogous.) Then when there appears the pronoun _``ji''_ (``me'' in English) the effect is as if the words ``Jim Carter'' had been written in its place. That is, ``A rat ate my cheese'' and ``A rat ate Jim Carter's cheese'' mean exactly the same thing. +For example, a document typically will have a signature line saying in effect this text is the output of Jim Carter. (Spoken discourse is completely analogous.) Then when there appears the pronoun _ji_ (me in English) the effect is as if the words Jim Carter had been written in its place. That is, A rat ate my cheese and A rat ate Jim Carter's cheese mean exactly the same thing. Phrases attached to a pronoun replace or supplement phrases in its antecedent. In general, if two arguments are given for the same case, numbered or modal, it is not an error but the first is ignored. ### Phrase-Relative Pronouns -_Gua\spi_ has phrase-relative pronouns, question pronouns, names and modal pronouns. Phrase-relative pronouns are for copying neighboring phrases --- arguments or entire sentences. Each point of use may have a different neighbor, so the same word used in different places may have different antecedents, while to reach the same antecedent from different places may require different words. The pronouns whose antecedent is a whole phrase are automatically infinitives; an explicit _``vo''_ is not needed. The most often used such pronouns are: +_Gua\spi_ has phrase-relative pronouns, question pronouns, names and modal pronouns. Phrase-relative pronouns are for copying neighboring phrases --- arguments or entire sentences. Each point of use may have a different neighbor, so the same word used in different places may have different antecedents, while to reach the same antecedent from different places may require different words. The pronouns whose antecedent is a whole phrase are automatically infinitives; an explicit _vo_ is not needed. The most often used such pronouns are: > @@ -117,7 +117,7 @@ _Gua\spi_ has phrase-relative pronouns, question pronouns, names and modal prono -There are pronouns for five cases and the whole phrase, for five phrase types --- 30 in all. The complete list is under [Vocabulary: Phrase-Relative Pronouns](../vocab1.html#pronouns). Phrase-relative pronouns like ``it'' are common in English, but in _gua\spi_ it is easier to redesignate an argument, so phrase-relative pronouns are less used in _gua\spi_. +There are pronouns for five cases and the whole phrase, for five phrase types --- 30 in all. The complete list is under [Vocabulary: Phrase-Relative Pronouns](../vocab1.html#pronouns). Phrase-relative pronouns like it are common in English, but in _gua\spi_ it is easier to redesignate an argument, so phrase-relative pronouns are less used in _gua\spi_. ### Question Pronouns @@ -320,7 +320,7 @@ For question pronouns the listener is supposed to say the antecedent; in other w ### Names -In _gua\spi_ a name is a pronoun. A name consists of a predicate prefixed by _``qu''_ , or _``qo''_ for foreign names, which disconnects the usual meaning of the predicate and substitutes the pronoun behavior. People are assigned permanent names at birth through a performative (ritual) statement like this: +In _gua\spi_ a name is a pronoun. A name consists of a predicate prefixed by _qu_ , or _qo_ for foreign names, which disconnects the usual meaning of the predicate and substitutes the pronoun behavior. People are assigned permanent names at birth through a performative (ritual) statement like this: > @@ -346,17 +346,17 @@ In _gua\spi_ a name is a pronoun. A name consists of a predicate prefixed by _`` -From then on, _``!jw |cil''_ (``this child'', with context so the listeners remember which one) is the antecedent of the name _``qo -ben''_. Subsequently when Ben is introduced to people a speaker will make a similar statement, except lacking ``_tri_ -ritual'' so it is constative (a statement of fact). Either statement lets the listener know what is the antecedent of the pronoun _``qo -ben''_. The antecedent then has a referent, which is the actual person being named. +From then on, _!jw |cil_ (this child, with context so the listeners remember which one) is the antecedent of the name _qo -ben_. Subsequently when Ben is introduced to people a speaker will make a similar statement, except lacking _tri_ -ritual so it is constative (a statement of fact). Either statement lets the listener know what is the antecedent of the pronoun _qo -ben_. The antecedent then has a referent, which is the actual person being named. -The meaning (if any) of the predicate without _``qu''_ is not changed. There is no implication that the antecedent may fit in the first case of the name, e.g. ``Mr. Coward'' may be quite courageous. A single referent can, and usually does, have several names. Semantics, though perhaps not law, allows you to put a new antecedent on a name at any time. +The meaning (if any) of the predicate without _qu_ is not changed. There is no implication that the antecedent may fit in the first case of the name, e.g. Mr. Coward may be quite courageous. A single referent can, and usually does, have several names. Semantics, though perhaps not law, allows you to put a new antecedent on a name at any time. The six variables _da, de, di, do, du, dy_ are names which you can assign to important concepts in nonfiction or characters in fiction. In mathematics it is common to use letter words as names for mathematical expressions, which in _gua\spi_ have the form of arguments. When the name antecedent, or any pronoun antecedent, is assigned all pronouns therein are already replaced by their antecedents, so that if those contained pronouns are later reassigned the name antecedent is unchanged. ### Personal Pronouns and Modal Defaults -Modal cases have a default value which is inserted in each sentence lacking explicit words for that case. Let us again use the speaker's modal case as an example. Most or all sentences in the document have the same speaker even though no speaker clause appears on them explicitly. There is a default value for the speaker, which is like a pronoun in that the antecedent words of the default are placed automatically into every sentence lacking an explicit speaker clause. The same default is the antecedent of the personal pronoun ``_ji_ -me''. The subordinate clause to identify the speaker initially is _``|qi -jai !X''_ ; the _``qi''_ prefix indicates that the default modal case for the speaker should be changed to X. The default value is like a pronoun, in that the antecedent words of the default are placed into the sentences. +Modal cases have a default value which is inserted in each sentence lacking explicit words for that case. Let us again use the speaker's modal case as an example. Most or all sentences in the document have the same speaker even though no speaker clause appears on them explicitly. There is a default value for the speaker, which is like a pronoun in that the antecedent words of the default are placed automatically into every sentence lacking an explicit speaker clause. The same default is the antecedent of the personal pronoun _ji_ -me. The subordinate clause to identify the speaker initially is _|qi -jai !X_ ; the _qi_ prefix indicates that the default modal case for the speaker should be changed to X. The default value is like a pronoun, in that the antecedent words of the default are placed into the sentences. -``Personal'' pronouns, so called because ``me'' and ``you'' are among them, mostly represent certain important modal case defaults. There have already been several examples. The personal pronouns are: +Personal pronouns, so called because me and you are among them, mostly represent certain important modal case defaults. There have already been several examples. The personal pronouns are: > @@ -478,9 +478,9 @@ Modal cases have a default value which is inserted in each sentence lacking expl -_``jy''_ marks the open case of an argument, where the referent goes. It appears by default in the first case, but you may link something else there explicitly with _``so''_ and put _``jy''_ explicitly elsewhere. Conversion is neater. +_jy_ marks the open case of an argument, where the referent goes. It appears by default in the first case, but you may link something else there explicitly with _so_ and put _jy_ explicitly elsewhere. Conversion is neater. -The modal case default has a kind of stack, so that a previous default value can be saved, replaced, and later restored. Here is a list of the control prefixes for the modal cases and their stacks. Except _``ql''_ , each must be used in front of a predicate, which identifies which modal case is being affected. +The modal case default has a kind of stack, so that a previous default value can be saved, replaced, and later restored. Here is a list of the control prefixes for the modal cases and their stacks. Except _ql_ , each must be used in front of a predicate, which identifies which modal case is being affected. > @@ -563,7 +563,11 @@ Here is an example of stacked cases, in story dialog: @@ -573,7 +577,10 @@ Here is an example of stacked cases, in story dialog: @@ -583,7 +590,10 @@ Here is an example of stacked cases, in story dialog: @@ -602,23 +612,23 @@ Here is an example of stacked cases, in story dialog: -Kira said the first three sentences, and the effect is as if the words _``|jai !qo -kira''_ were added to the second and third, while the narrator said the last one, and _``qa''_ restores him as speaker. +Kira said the first three sentences, and the effect is as if the words _|jai !qo -kira_ were added to the second and third, while the narrator said the last one, and _qa_ restores him as speaker. -Tenses are also handled this way, as is any modal case. If you put _``|qe -cnu !X''_ on the opening descriptive sentence (where X is an event identifying when the sentence happens) then it will be propagated to subsequent sentences automatically --- unlike in English where a syntactically complicated and less precise tense has to be used on every sentence. John Parks-Clifford, then with the Loglan Institute, originally developed this concept of tense defaults [TL43]. +Tenses are also handled this way, as is any modal case. If you put _|qe -cnu !X_ on the opening descriptive sentence (where X is an event identifying when the sentence happens) then it will be propagated to subsequent sentences automatically --- unlike in English where a syntactically complicated and less precise tense has to be used on every sentence. John Parks-Clifford, then with the Loglan Institute, originally developed this concept of tense defaults [TL43]. -In the third dialog line above, notice the use of ``_jo_ -you'' to make the sentence imperative. The decoration ``_|vi-pli_ -please'' shows the speaker's attitude but does not by itself make the sentence imperative. +In the third dialog line above, notice the use of _jo_ -you to make the sentence imperative. The decoration _|vi-pli_ -please shows the speaker's attitude but does not by itself make the sentence imperative. ## Compound Predicates -A key organizational element of _gua\spi_ is the compound word, a sequence of predicates and structure words. The first word's tone shows where the compound attaches. There may be several following words in the compound, each of which has high even tone `-' or low even tone `='. A phrase predicate often begins with prefixes, such as the caselink _``sa''_ or the conversion _``zu''_ , but the most significant use of compounding is to join two or more predicate words. +A key organizational element of _gua\spi_ is the compound word, a sequence of predicates and structure words. The first word's tone shows where the compound attaches. There may be several following words in the compound, each of which has high even tone `-' or low even tone `='. A phrase predicate often begins with prefixes, such as the caselink _sa_ or the conversion _zu_ , but the most significant use of compounding is to join two or more predicate words. -The motivation to make compounds is twofold. First, you can use a single argument list to say what amounts to two sentences, which when compounded are much easier for the listener to interpret. Second, just as we use Latin prefixes in English to make many words from one, e.g. ``ob-ject'', ``pro-ject'', ``in-ject'', ``ab-ject'', most meanings in _gua\spi_ are achieved by combining a much broader range of predicates. A beginner can learn the primitive words, about 1400, and then stick them together in self-created compounds which he can expect any listener to understand, while to achieve the same range of expression in natural languages the speaker and the listener must master a huge vocabulary in which most of the words are rarely used. +The motivation to make compounds is twofold. First, you can use a single argument list to say what amounts to two sentences, which when compounded are much easier for the listener to interpret. Second, just as we use Latin prefixes in English to make many words from one, e.g. ob-ject, pro-ject, in-ject, ab-ject, most meanings in _gua\spi_ are achieved by combining a much broader range of predicates. A beginner can learn the primitive words, about 1400, and then stick them together in self-created compounds which he can expect any listener to understand, while to achieve the same range of expression in natural languages the speaker and the listener must master a huge vocabulary in which most of the words are rarely used. Though humans like to think of compound predicates as separate words analogous to the primitive words, compounds are actually defined through the transformations described below, so that each primitive word heads a separate phrase. For example in a transitive compound the compounded object is to be taken off and put in its proper case as a sub-phrase. Thus one can easily and reliably interpret a compound word that one has never heard before, as long as one knows all the primitive words. ### Infinitive Argument -There are three main patterns to the compounds. First, if the main word has a case with a default linker of _``vo''_ or _``bi''_ \--- that is, a case for an infinitive --- a word compounded with high even tone `-' is the predicate of that infinitive, and the main word case before the infinitive (before conversion) becomes the infinitive's first case. (Exceptions are noted in the dictionary.) Here are examples: +There are three main patterns to the compounds. First, if the main word has a case with a default linker of _vo_ or _bi_ \--- that is, a case for an infinitive --- a word compounded with high even tone `-' is the predicate of that infinitive, and the main word case before the infinitive (before conversion) becomes the infinitive's first case. (Exceptions are noted in the dictionary.) Here are examples: > @@ -909,11 +919,11 @@ There are three main patterns to the compounds. First, if the main word has a ca -The patterns with ``_can_ -change'' and ``_qma_ -make'' are as common in _gua\spi_ as the pre-pro-in-ob-ab Latin prefix set is in English. In the second and third examples, the infinitive first case is a copy of the main sentence case before the infinitive: the second case. _Gua\spi_ predicate words are usually not transitive, and the third example shows how transitive predicates are produced. (There is a separate word ``_fow_ -force''.) As shown in the fourth example, multiple compounds are common. The expansion of this sentence is also shown with all defaults written out in full. +The patterns with _can_ -change and _qma_ -make are as common in _gua\spi_ as the pre-pro-in-ob-ab Latin prefix set is in English. In the second and third examples, the infinitive first case is a copy of the main sentence case before the infinitive: the second case. _Gua\spi_ predicate words are usually not transitive, and the third example shows how transitive predicates are produced. (There is a separate word _fow_ -force.) As shown in the fourth example, multiple compounds are common. The expansion of this sentence is also shown with all defaults written out in full. ### Shared Argument List -Second, the words may share an argument list. The effect is as if you had made two sentences with the arguments copied into each. This pattern is cued by `-' when the infinitive argument pattern does not apply, or by a conjunction _``-fe''_ when it does. For example: +Second, the words may share an argument list. The effect is as if you had made two sentences with the arguments copied into each. This pattern is cued by `-' when the infinitive argument pattern does not apply, or by a conjunction _-fe_ when it does. For example: > @@ -967,7 +977,9 @@ Second, the words may share an argument list. The effect is as if you had made t @@ -1044,13 +1056,13 @@ Second, the words may share an argument list. The effect is as if you had made t -The first sentence shows this pattern plainly. In the second, the predicate of an argument is compound and the pronoun _``jy''_ is used in the paired expansions to represent its open first case. It is very common for a motion word and a directional property to share arguments, as in the last three sentences. In the fourth one, with a transitive motion word the directional property relates the thing that moves, not the actor, to the destination. The polarity of the direction is often reverse of English: ``swims down'' means it is ``_qmy_ -above'' the destination. A better translation is ``swims from above''. +The first sentence shows this pattern plainly. In the second, the predicate of an argument is compound and the pronoun _jy_ is used in the paired expansions to represent its open first case. It is very common for a motion word and a directional property to share arguments, as in the last three sentences. In the fourth one, with a transitive motion word the directional property relates the thing that moves, not the actor, to the destination. The polarity of the direction is often reverse of English: swims down means it is _qmy_ -above the destination. A better translation is swims from above. Most usually the first and second cases of all the predicates go together. For exceptions as in the fourth sentence above the dictionary shows which cases merge. ### Object of Transitive Predicate -A third pattern is found in which a transitive main word is followed by its object as a compound. It is cued by the tone `='. In this pattern the main word's second case (before conversion) receives the sub-word as an argument, except that if the main word is converted so the second case is unavailable, the first case (before conversion) gets the sub-word. For organizing the other arguments the effect is the same as if an explicit caselink _``!su''_ had been used. For example: +A third pattern is found in which a transitive main word is followed by its object as a compound. It is cued by the tone `='. In this pattern the main word's second case (before conversion) receives the sub-word as an argument, except that if the main word is converted so the second case is unavailable, the first case (before conversion) gets the sub-word. For organizing the other arguments the effect is the same as if an explicit caselink _!su_ had been used. For example: > @@ -1149,11 +1161,11 @@ A third pattern is found in which a transitive main word is followed by its obje -_``dri -fli''_ would be the second type of compound, meaning that its referent both drives and flies, like a bird driving a car. This is not quite the right meaning. +_dri -fli_ would be the second type of compound, meaning that its referent both drives and flies, like a bird driving a car. This is not quite the right meaning. ### Foreign Words and Metaphors -The prefixes _``fo''_ , _``fn''_ and _``qo''_ start a foreign word, and all subsequent compounded words are part of it. The word must start with a C and end with a V, and so you may have to modify the word either by removing or adding letters. Put a glottal stop before an initial vowel. Sounds not in _gua\spi_ are mangled to fit. _``qo''_ is for foreign names, _``fo''_ marks a non-name predicate, and _``fn''_ is for foreign metaphors. As a predicate the foreign word means ``X1 is (whatever)'' but listeners rarely know its meaning, so cases are never recognized on a foreign word. Normally you attach it as a metaphor to a _gua\spi_ predicate, both to provide cases and to give the listener a cue, if not to the exact meaning, at least to the category of the meaning. For example, +The prefixes _fo_ , _fn_ and _qo_ start a foreign word, and all subsequent compounded words are part of it. The word must start with a C and end with a V, and so you may have to modify the word either by removing or adding letters. Put a glottal stop before an initial vowel. Sounds not in _gua\spi_ are mangled to fit. _qo_ is for foreign names, _fo_ marks a non-name predicate, and _fn_ is for foreign metaphors. As a predicate the foreign word means X1 is (whatever) but listeners rarely know its meaning, so cases are never recognized on a foreign word. Normally you attach it as a metaphor to a _gua\spi_ predicate, both to provide cases and to give the listener a cue, if not to the exact meaning, at least to the category of the meaning. For example, > @@ -1189,13 +1201,13 @@ The prefixes _``fo''_ , _``fn''_ and _``qo''_ start a foreign word, and all subs -Here the foreign name ``John'' could be pronounced without change (note the phonetic spelling), but ``australopithecine'' needed work. While in English we use the adjective or set-membership form of the word, in _gua\spi_ it works better to use the root form of the foreign word, if you know it: ``australopithecus''. A glottal stop `:' was added at the beginning, `th' was changed to `t', the final consonant `s' was removed (a V could have been added), and the word was stuck on ``_dwu_ -animal'' to provide cases. This long word has five _gua\spi_ syllables, treated in morphology as separate words, and they are stuck together by compounding tone `-'. (Normally the `-' tone symbols are not written inside a foreign word.) +Here the foreign name John could be pronounced without change (note the phonetic spelling), but australopithecine needed work. While in English we use the adjective or set-membership form of the word, in _gua\spi_ it works better to use the root form of the foreign word, if you know it: australopithecus. A glottal stop `:' was added at the beginning, `th' was changed to `t', the final consonant `s' was removed (a V could have been added), and the word was stuck on _dwu_ -animal to provide cases. This long word has five _gua\spi_ syllables, treated in morphology as separate words, and they are stuck together by compounding tone `-'. (Normally the `-' tone symbols are not written inside a foreign word.) -Similarly on the other sentence, _gua\spi_ predicates are put in front of the foreign words to help the listener interpret them. An educated human knows what a Borneo is, but a naive listener, particularly mechanical, needs the assistance of ``_dlau_ -island''. +Similarly on the other sentence, _gua\spi_ predicates are put in front of the foreign words to help the listener interpret them. An educated human knows what a Borneo is, but a naive listener, particularly mechanical, needs the assistance of _dlau_ -island. Foreign words make the organization ambiguous: when a word has compounding tone is it compound or does it continue a foreign word? The Procrustean method is used to resolve the ambiguity: a foreign word eats all following first tone words. -While metaphors are most common with foreign words, they are also available with _gua\spi_ predicates, being cued by the conjunction _``fw''_. They resemble shared-argument compounds but the meaning is not so precisely derived from the combined words. Only the cases of the main word appear; the sub-word's cases are ignored. (But metaphors are clearer if the first cases of all the words can merge.) Use regular compounds if at all possible, as in the last example, because they are unambiguous. +While metaphors are most common with foreign words, they are also available with _gua\spi_ predicates, being cued by the conjunction _fw_. They resemble shared-argument compounds but the meaning is not so precisely derived from the combined words. Only the cases of the main word appear; the sub-word's cases are ignored. (But metaphors are clearer if the first cases of all the words can merge.) Use regular compounds if at all possible, as in the last example, because they are unambiguous. > @@ -1276,7 +1288,7 @@ While metaphors are most common with foreign words, they are also available with ### Special Cases -Numbers are a special case in organization. Compounded digits after the first build up a multi-digit cardinal number, with a possible sign, exponent and decimal point. Quasidigits, which appear first, mean ``approximately'', ``at least'', and so on. As a predicate a cardinal number means ``X1 is a set of N members, one of which is X2''. An ordinal number, cued by the quasidigit _``tr''_ , means ``X1 is N'th in list (_xy_) X2 starting at X3''. For example: +Numbers are a special case in organization. Compounded digits after the first build up a multi-digit cardinal number, with a possible sign, exponent and decimal point. Quasidigits, which appear first, mean approximately, at least, and so on. As a predicate a cardinal number means ``X1 is a set of N members, one of which is X2''. An ordinal number, cued by the quasidigit _tr_ , means ``X1 is N'th in list (_xy_) X2 starting at X3''. For example: > diff --git a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/semantic.md b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/semantic.md index 72611b5b0..b0f0cf7a6 100644 --- a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/semantic.md +++ b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/semantic.md @@ -35,16 +35,16 @@ As stated earlier, a predicate word expresses a relation between the occupants of its cases, and is defined by a referent set consisting of lists of case occupants that are thus related. -To interpret an argument, you start with its predicate's referent set. You retain members consistent with any sub-phrases. From each member you extract the first case occupant, and out of these you make the ``full referent set'' of the argument. (The full referent set is empty in important special cases.) The ``referent subset'', which is the set of actual referents of the argument, is a subset of the full set which depends on a prefix word called an ``article''. +To interpret an argument, you start with its predicate's referent set. You retain members consistent with any sub-phrases. From each member you extract the first case occupant, and out of these you make the full referent set of the argument. (The full referent set is empty in important special cases.) The referent subset, which is the set of actual referents of the argument, is a subset of the full set which depends on a prefix word called an article. -``Consistent with sub-phrases'' means this: the n'th case occupant of each main predicate referent set member must be in the referent subset of the n'th case sub-argument. In addition, if the sub-argument has a prefix ``_ve_ -each'', then make equivalence classes of main referent set members (that survive all restrictions simultaneously) which differ only in the n'th case. If the set of n'th case occupants from an equivalence class is not equal to the sub-argument referent subset (i.e. it is not true that each sub-argument referent is in the equivalence class), reject the whole equivalence class. For subordinate clauses, the same procedure applies except the predicate is from the clause and the restricted phrase acts as one of its sub-arguments. +Consistent with sub-phrases means this: the n'th case occupant of each main predicate referent set member must be in the referent subset of the n'th case sub-argument. In addition, if the sub-argument has a prefix _ve_ -each, then make equivalence classes of main referent set members (that survive all restrictions simultaneously) which differ only in the n'th case. If the set of n'th case occupants from an equivalence class is not equal to the sub-argument referent subset (i.e. it is not true that each sub-argument referent is in the equivalence class), reject the whole equivalence class. For subordinate clauses, the same procedure applies except the predicate is from the clause and the restricted phrase acts as one of its sub-arguments. ## The Articles Here are the articles. There are two articles for each meaning; the first unfolds the referent subset so each member is a referent, while the second specifies that the referent is the referent subset as a set. The careful distinction between sets and extensions of their members is characteristic of _gua\spi_. -_``xe''_ , _``xy''_ : - The most common article is _``xe''_ , and it is assumed with most predicates when arguments lack an article. Its English translation is ``the''. The referent subset is whichever members the speaker has in mind to talk about, but generally there are prior context cues to show which out of numerous possibilities are intended as the referents. In particular, if a set of referents has been designated before and if it is the only such set that is a subset of the full referent set of the argument, then those are the referents of the argument. For example, +_xe_ , _xy_ : + The most common article is _xe_ , and it is assumed with most predicates when arguments lack an article. Its English translation is the. The referent subset is whichever members the speaker has in mind to talk about, but generally there are prior context cues to show which out of numerous possibilities are intended as the referents. In particular, if a set of referents has been designated before and if it is the only such set that is a subset of the full referent set of the argument, then those are the referents of the argument. For example, > @@ -80,13 +80,13 @@ _``xe''_ , _``xy''_ : -``_fkar_ -car'' appears three times; in each instances its article is ``_xe_ -the'' but the article is unseen, being provided by default. The first instance of ``_fkar_ -car'' designates one referent in detail. The other two instances are typical arguments with ``_xe_ -the'': since the prior referent fits this predicate (and in the second sentence ``its garage'' does not), the prior referent is being redesignated. Because _gua\spi_ words are so short it is just as efficient to redesignate an argument like this as to use a phrase-relative pronoun, so pronouns are less commonly used in _gua\spi_ than in English. +_fkar_ -car appears three times; in each instances its article is _xe_ -the but the article is unseen, being provided by default. The first instance of _fkar_ -car designates one referent in detail. The other two instances are typical arguments with _xe_ -the: since the prior referent fits this predicate (and in the second sentence its garage does not), the prior referent is being redesignated. Because _gua\spi_ words are so short it is just as efficient to redesignate an argument like this as to use a phrase-relative pronoun, so pronouns are less commonly used in _gua\spi_ than in English. -_``xa''_ , _``xu''_ : - The subset equals the full referent set. The referents are everything that fits the predicate. Mathematical expressions almost invariably have _``xu''_ as the article, as in _``xu -cu''_ = ``the equivalence class of all pairs'' = ``the number two''. -_``xi''_ , _``xr''_ : - Only a few members are left out, which the speaker has in mind as being ``atypical''. By judiciously using _``xi''_ the speaker can prune out excessive special cases from his discussion. But unfortunately listeners have a lot of trouble to identify correctly which members are being left out. Beware. -_``xo''_ , _``xw''_ : +_xa_ , _xu_ : + The subset equals the full referent set. The referents are everything that fits the predicate. Mathematical expressions almost invariably have _xu_ as the article, as in _``xu -cu''_ = the equivalence class of all pairs = the number two. +_xi_ , _xr_ : + Only a few members are left out, which the speaker has in mind as being atypical. By judiciously using _xi_ the speaker can prune out excessive special cases from his discussion. But unfortunately listeners have a lot of trouble to identify correctly which members are being left out. Beware. +_xo_ , _xw_ : From the full referent set one or more members are selected, and it doesn't matter which ones. For example, > @@ -113,9 +113,9 @@ _``xo''_ , _``xw''_ : -All in the box are equivalent and it doesn't matter which you get. _``xo''_ is often used for arguments in the ``serving or portion'' category, called ``partitive nouns'' in English. +All in the box are equivalent and it doesn't matter which you get. _xo_ is often used for arguments in the ``serving or portion'' category, called partitive nouns in English. -_``xn''_ : +_xn_ : No members of the full referent set are in the referent subset. This article is useful for negative statements like > @@ -153,11 +153,11 @@ _``xn''_ : -Actually _``xn''_ makes a statement about the excluded referent set members; were it regarded as a statement about all the members of an empty set, there could be no examples or counterexamples and the sentence would assert nothing. The second example is the contrapositive of the first, and shows what _``xn''_ really means. +Actually _xn_ makes a statement about the excluded referent set members; were it regarded as a statement about all the members of an empty set, there could be no examples or counterexamples and the sentence would assert nothing. The second example is the contrapositive of the first, and shows what _xn_ really means. -Each case of each predicate has a default prefix, used with any arguments that do not have explicit articles. Most of these defaults are _``xe''_ ; _``xu''_ is used with math operators because of the meaning of numbers; _``xy''_ is used for cases that need sets; _``xe vo''_ is common for words that deal with events; and _``xe bi''_ appears in a few cases that need texts. In the latter two cases appearance of any article cancels both _``xe''_ and _``vo''_ or _``bi''_. _``xy''_ defaults have a special behavior: they change to _``xe''_ if the argument is already a set. +Each case of each predicate has a default prefix, used with any arguments that do not have explicit articles. Most of these defaults are _xe_ ; _xu_ is used with math operators because of the meaning of numbers; _xy_ is used for cases that need sets; _xe vo_ is common for words that deal with events; and _xe bi_ appears in a few cases that need texts. In the latter two cases appearance of any article cancels both _xe_ and _vo_ or _bi_. _xy_ defaults have a special behavior: they change to _xe_ if the argument is already a set. -When an argument predicate has a default article for its first case other than _``xe''_ , and when it is in a case with a _``xe''_ default, the argument predicate's article is used. Otherwise if the defaults are unequal and not _``xe''_ , the situation is ambiguous and the speaker is required to say the article he wants. +When an argument predicate has a default article for its first case other than _xe_ , and when it is in a case with a _xe_ default, the argument predicate's article is used. Otherwise if the defaults are unequal and not _xe_ , the situation is ambiguous and the speaker is required to say the article he wants. ## Cartesian Expansion of Arguments @@ -197,7 +197,7 @@ The reason arguments are in a phrase is to select particular events from the def -The second example could easily be interpreted the same as the first, but if one knows that particular events are not in the definition of the predicate --- cows do not ``_crw_ -eat'' rats --- one will normally discount such terms in the Cartesian expansion without thought. The arguments then merely select pre-known events from the definition. If the speaker wishes to assert that a cow ate a rat, he should put the unusual event in a separate sentence and should emphasize it. +The second example could easily be interpreted the same as the first, but if one knows that particular events are not in the definition of the predicate --- cows do not _crw_ -eat rats --- one will normally discount such terms in the Cartesian expansion without thought. The arguments then merely select pre-known events from the definition. If the speaker wishes to assert that a cow ate a rat, he should put the unusual event in a separate sentence and should emphasize it. Often within a sentence --- this applies to top-level sentences, not just phrases --- the same argument appears in several cases, either as a pronoun or explicitly redesignated. In each member of the referent set of the sentence the occupants of the replicated case are equal --- they are not selected independently from the referent sets of the replicated argument, as they would be if the arguments were different. Sub-phrases restrict main phrase events one by one, and the occupant of a replicated case in the sub-phrase is also equal to the one in the main phrase. For example: @@ -237,7 +237,7 @@ Often within a sentence --- this applies to top-level sentences, not just phrase -There is one exception: if an argument is prefixed by _``vl''_ , its referent is chosen independently of any other copies of it. _``vr''_ as a prefix means ``_inter alia_ '' or ``each other''. Its meaning is like _``vl''_ except that in the sentence's referent set the prefixed argument's referent may not duplicate that of other copies of the same argument. +There is one exception: if an argument is prefixed by _vl_ , its referent is chosen independently of any other copies of it. _vr_ as a prefix means _inter alia_ or each other. Its meaning is like _vl_ except that in the sentence's referent set the prefixed argument's referent may not duplicate that of other copies of the same argument. > @@ -264,9 +264,9 @@ There is one exception: if an argument is prefixed by _``vl''_ , its referent is -If an argument prefixed by _``vr''_ or _``vl''_ is replicated by a pronoun the same referent is used both places, but if the pronoun has its own prefix that one supercedes any in the antecedent and the referent is chosen independently. +If an argument prefixed by _vr_ or _vl_ is replicated by a pronoun the same referent is used both places, but if the pronoun has its own prefix that one supercedes any in the antecedent and the referent is chosen independently. -In addition, the sentence start words _``:u''_ and _``:o''_ mean that the sentence should be interpreted coordinately with the one before it. The referent sets of the two sentence predicates are multiplied in the sense of Descartes and the argument set members are fitted into their respective cases the same as in a single sentence. In particular, arguments replicated in both sentences have the replicated cases equal in each referent set member. +In addition, the sentence start words _:u_ and _:o_ mean that the sentence should be interpreted coordinately with the one before it. The referent sets of the two sentence predicates are multiplied in the sense of Descartes and the argument set members are fitted into their respective cases the same as in a single sentence. In particular, arguments replicated in both sentences have the replicated cases equal in each referent set member. > @@ -295,9 +295,9 @@ In addition, the sentence start words _``:u''_ and _``:o''_ mean that the senten -The same statement is shown twice; the first one uses a pronoun while the second repeats the argument _``ftu =plyw''_ explicitly. +The same statement is shown twice; the first one uses a pronoun while the second repeats the argument _ftu =plyw_ explicitly. -For statements of logic one may produce a nonsentence containing the arguments to be discussed, called ``prenex arguments'', and then state one or more sentences about them, starting each sentence with _``^:u''_ and repeating the arguments or using pronouns. Since the Cartesian product is not commutative when negation is involved, or with mixed universal and existential quantification, the speaker may be forced to produce arguments in an inconvenient order, and prenex arguments may sound better. Here is an example of prenex arguments: +For statements of logic one may produce a nonsentence containing the arguments to be discussed, called prenex arguments, and then state one or more sentences about them, starting each sentence with _^:u_ and repeating the arguments or using pronouns. Since the Cartesian product is not commutative when negation is involved, or with mixed universal and existential quantification, the speaker may be forced to produce arguments in an inconvenient order, and prenex arguments may sound better. Here is an example of prenex arguments: > @@ -333,7 +333,7 @@ For statements of logic one may produce a nonsentence containing the arguments t -A conjunction is a pair or list of similar grammatical structures which act as if they were one unit. We have seen the conjunction word _``fe''_ in compound words, but it also works with arguments to combine several into one, just as when one argument designates several referents. Here is a tricky example from before: +A conjunction is a pair or list of similar grammatical structures which act as if they were one unit. We have seen the conjunction word _fe_ in compound words, but it also works with arguments to combine several into one, just as when one argument designates several referents. Here is a tricky example from before: > @@ -369,13 +369,13 @@ A conjunction is a pair or list of similar grammatical structures which act as i -The two argument pairs are Cartesian multiplied to select referent set members of ``_crw_ -eat'' just as in the second example the two arguments designate multiple referents which are Cartesian multiplied. See also [Vocabulary: Conjunctions](../vocab1.html#conjunctions) for other kinds of conjunctions. +The two argument pairs are Cartesian multiplied to select referent set members of _crw_ -eat just as in the second example the two arguments designate multiple referents which are Cartesian multiplied. See also [Vocabulary: Conjunctions](../vocab1.html#conjunctions) for other kinds of conjunctions. ## Miscellany about Arguments -Formally the articles are described as prefixes, but it makes sense semantically to regard them as predicates with two arguments; a full referent set comes in (as a set) to the second argument and the appropriate referent subset comes out in the first. With this definition, for example, _``xa''_ (full referent set in extension) would mean exactly the same as ``_xor_ -set member''. It is easier that way to interpret compound articles like _``xo -xi''_ = ``any typical X''. However, articles have to be prefixes grammatically so the argument's main predicate can have its own arguments before it as well as after. +Formally the articles are described as prefixes, but it makes sense semantically to regard them as predicates with two arguments; a full referent set comes in (as a set) to the second argument and the appropriate referent subset comes out in the first. With this definition, for example, _xa_ (full referent set in extension) would mean exactly the same as _xor_ -set member. It is easier that way to interpret compound articles like _xo -xi_ = any typical X. However, articles have to be prefixes grammatically so the argument's main predicate can have its own arguments before it as well as after. -Most Indo-European languages distinguish between genders and numbers of arguments. Like Chinese and English, _gua\spi_ has no gender, though you may use a subordinate clause like ``_|fmy_ -female''. Number comes from the referent sets, not the grammar. You may specify the exact number of referents with a numeric predicate, like this: +Most Indo-European languages distinguish between genders and numbers of arguments. Like Chinese and English, _gua\spi_ has no gender, though you may use a subordinate clause like _|fmy_ -female. Number comes from the referent sets, not the grammar. You may specify the exact number of referents with a numeric predicate, like this: > diff --git a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/vocab1.md b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/vocab1.md index c47fa58eb..bea719b43 100644 --- a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/vocab1.md +++ b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/vocab1.md @@ -39,9 +39,9 @@ A major difference between _gua\spi_ and Old _Loglan_ (and, I fear, _Lojban_) is ## Functional Categories of Words -_Gua\spi_ words have a relation between function and morphology. The CV pattern is for structure words, a few pronouns, and digits. CVV is for ``real'' relations, what in English are verbs. CCV is for abstract ``nouns'', normally used to denote objects. CCVV is used for more concrete ``nouns'' such as species, chemical elements, or household artifacts. The purpose of these assignments is to cater to the known proclivities of natural language speakers, who like to segregate nouns from verbs, and to simplify the process of making words for meanings. Nothing in the language depends on these assignments, and the language never actually distinguishes between nouns and verbs. If you find that some word has a morphological form other than what you expect, this is simply an effort to keep related words together, and it will have no effect on the efficiency of the language. +_Gua\spi_ words have a relation between function and morphology. The CV pattern is for structure words, a few pronouns, and digits. CVV is for real relations, what in English are verbs. CCV is for abstract nouns, normally used to denote objects. CCVV is used for more concrete nouns such as species, chemical elements, or household artifacts. The purpose of these assignments is to cater to the known proclivities of natural language speakers, who like to segregate nouns from verbs, and to simplify the process of making words for meanings. Nothing in the language depends on these assignments, and the language never actually distinguishes between nouns and verbs. If you find that some word has a morphological form other than what you expect, this is simply an effort to keep related words together, and it will have no effect on the efficiency of the language. -Here is the distribution of _gua\spi_ words by functional and morphological category. There are 11 V's, and 14 regular C's. `:' is allowed only in CV words such as _``^:i''_ , and `#' does not count towards differences in words. There are 74 allowed CC digraphs out of 196 possible pairs; many CC's are too hard for people to recognize reliably. +Here is the distribution of _gua\spi_ words by functional and morphological category. There are 11 V's, and 14 regular C's. `:' is allowed only in CV words such as _^:i_ , and `#' does not count towards differences in words. There are 74 allowed CC digraphs out of 196 possible pairs; many CC's are too hard for people to recognize reliably. > @@ -272,9 +272,9 @@ Because the words are modestly similar to their natural language counterparts, l CV structure words were assigned by hand; related structure words, like articles, have the same C and varying V's. Structure words pertaining to numbered cases have the same V's as the corresponding digits, but contrasting consonants, making learning easier. -A question often asked is, why create new words? Why not use Chinese or English words? First, some attempt has been made to keep _gua\spi_ culturally neutral, and if Chinese words were used it would intimidate English speakers and vice versa. More important, Chinese words are designed for use with Chinese. Many required meanings, like articles, simply do not exist in Chinese, and similarly in English. And those meanings that are present are only approximations of the _gua\spi_ meanings; while users have to invest a lot of effort to learn the new words and their definitions, they will find it even harder to keep straight what a word of their native language ``really'' means in _gua\spi_. That is why the approach was rejected of simply stealing natural language vocabulary. +A question often asked is, why create new words? Why not use Chinese or English words? First, some attempt has been made to keep _gua\spi_ culturally neutral, and if Chinese words were used it would intimidate English speakers and vice versa. More important, Chinese words are designed for use with Chinese. Many required meanings, like articles, simply do not exist in Chinese, and similarly in English. And those meanings that are present are only approximations of the _gua\spi_ meanings; while users have to invest a lot of effort to learn the new words and their definitions, they will find it even harder to keep straight what a word of their native language really means in _gua\spi_. That is why the approach was rejected of simply stealing natural language vocabulary. -Given some set, a ``basis'' is a subset from which all its members can be derived, as with vectors. Each vector space has a specific dimension, or number of basis elements, but words are not so simple. _Loglan_ has about 1000 primitive words and it was intended that virtually any meaning should be achievable as combinations of these words; that is, the primitive words form a basis of nearly all meanings. Experiment proved that this intention had been accomplished for the most part, but that as anticipated, some areas were incompletely or imprecisely covered. The _Lojban_ project of LeChevalier [Lja] is a continuation of Brown's work on _Loglan_ [L1], and he has added about 300 primitive words, mainly about human emotion and interaction. For _gua\spi_ I took over LeChevalier's primitive word list, with his kind assistance. I rewrote all the definitions to match _gua\spi_ usage. I also added and deleted a small number of words to deal with specific _gua\spi_ issues, and I expanded the scientific vocabulary in mathematics, chemistry, zoology, botany and agriculture. As a result, _gua\spi_ has about 1400 primitive words. Some people are interested to discover just how few basis words we can get by with. However, I have experience with the _Loglan_ word list and I have confidence in its ability to handle the required meanings; and while I do not believe it is minimal, I think it is fairly close. Thus I chose to use existing word lists for _gua\spi_ rather than to try for radical pruning or _de novo_ creation. +Given some set, a basis is a subset from which all its members can be derived, as with vectors. Each vector space has a specific dimension, or number of basis elements, but words are not so simple. _Loglan_ has about 1000 primitive words and it was intended that virtually any meaning should be achievable as combinations of these words; that is, the primitive words form a basis of nearly all meanings. Experiment proved that this intention had been accomplished for the most part, but that as anticipated, some areas were incompletely or imprecisely covered. The _Lojban_ project of LeChevalier [Lja] is a continuation of Brown's work on _Loglan_ [L1], and he has added about 300 primitive words, mainly about human emotion and interaction. For _gua\spi_ I took over LeChevalier's primitive word list, with his kind assistance. I rewrote all the definitions to match _gua\spi_ usage. I also added and deleted a small number of words to deal with specific _gua\spi_ issues, and I expanded the scientific vocabulary in mathematics, chemistry, zoology, botany and agriculture. As a result, _gua\spi_ has about 1400 primitive words. Some people are interested to discover just how few basis words we can get by with. However, I have experience with the _Loglan_ word list and I have confidence in its ability to handle the required meanings; and while I do not believe it is minimal, I think it is fairly close. Thus I chose to use existing word lists for _gua\spi_ rather than to try for radical pruning or _de novo_ creation. ## Words and Grammar @@ -400,7 +400,7 @@ The CCVV pattern is used for organized groups of noun-type words such as the phr -Here is a list of all the phrase-relative pronouns. There is also _``zgln''_ meaning ``the previous discourse in general'' and _``zglr''_ meaning ``the event just finished or still continuing''. +Here is a list of all the phrase-relative pronouns. There is also _zgln_ meaning the previous discourse in general and _zglr_ meaning the event just finished or still continuing. > @@ -612,7 +612,7 @@ Here is a list of all the phrase-relative pronouns. There is also _``zgln''_ mea ### Letterals -Letterals are words representing letters, which are built up on a regular pattern like the pronouns are. A letteral means ``X1 is an instance of the letter (whatever)''. _``zu -fma !xo - ''_ is the right way to say that something has the shape of a letter. To spell a word, compound the letters start to end. The result means ``X1 is an instance of something spelled (whatever)''. +Letterals are words representing letters, which are built up on a regular pattern like the pronouns are. A letteral means ``X1 is an instance of the letter (whatever)''. _zu -fma !xo - _ is the right way to say that something has the shape of a letter. To spell a word, compound the letters start to end. The result means ``X1 is an instance of something spelled (whatever)''. Among letterals there are different forms for V's and C's, and upper and lower case in two alphabets are supported. The first table shows stem forms arranged phonetically, while the second shows what to substitute for the asterisks to signal alphabets and cases. @@ -956,7 +956,7 @@ To select cases and alphabets, use these for * or **: -The relation of Greek letters to _gua\spi_ Roman letters is basically phonetic; the arbitrary assignments below are marked by `*'. The `:' can be used for the aspiration mark, an apostrophe in Greek or `h' in English transliteration, as in ``Hellas''. In principle one can also use a form like _``tler -fn -:alfa''_ (the letter alpha) to refer to Greek letters. +The relation of Greek letters to _gua\spi_ Roman letters is basically phonetic; the arbitrary assignments below are marked by `*'. The `:' can be used for the aspiration mark, an apostrophe in Greek or `h' in English transliteration, as in Hellas. In principle one can also use a form like _tler -fn -:alfa_ (the letter alpha) to refer to Greek letters. > @@ -1170,15 +1170,15 @@ In English we use acronyms freely, but in _gua\spi_ the letterals for the acrony ### Foreign Words -It is the policy in _gua\spi_ to use foreign words as-is (except for necessary mangling to make them fit the CV pattern), to represent the names of foreign people, places, flora, fauna, units of measure, foods, clothes, and so on. Certain of these word categories have a few members assigned CCV words. These are in _gua\spi_ because they were in _Loglan_ , and they were in _Loglan_ because they occurred with high frequency in European literature. In _Loglan_ it has proven impractical to manufacture in-language primitive words for every possible primitive, not for lack of word space but rather because those working on the language have other issues to attend to than making a continuous stream of predicates. For example, such an obvious animal as ``bear'' didn't make it into _Loglan_ and probably never will. _Gua\spi_ will have primitive words for more fauna and flora, but as in _Loglan_ , the majority of species will never have _gua\spi_ primitive words, and neither will most ethnic foods, foreign countries, provincial units and so on. +It is the policy in _gua\spi_ to use foreign words as-is (except for necessary mangling to make them fit the CV pattern), to represent the names of foreign people, places, flora, fauna, units of measure, foods, clothes, and so on. Certain of these word categories have a few members assigned CCV words. These are in _gua\spi_ because they were in _Loglan_ , and they were in _Loglan_ because they occurred with high frequency in European literature. In _Loglan_ it has proven impractical to manufacture in-language primitive words for every possible primitive, not for lack of word space but rather because those working on the language have other issues to attend to than making a continuous stream of predicates. For example, such an obvious animal as bear didn't make it into _Loglan_ and probably never will. _Gua\spi_ will have primitive words for more fauna and flora, but as in _Loglan_ , the majority of species will never have _gua\spi_ primitive words, and neither will most ethnic foods, foreign countries, provincial units and so on. -When foreign words fit neatly into the language, speakers are tempted to over-use them. Agglutinative languages like _gua\spi_ and _Loglan_ have great power to produce compound words with quite precise meanings, and speakers should try very hard to learn to produce such words. It is a fact that despite _Loglan_ 's limited set of nouns and, at that time, its lack of foreign words, I was able to write about 20,000 words of text while inventing only four unapproved primitive words (and a fifth was made for me after the fact): bear, torus, tape, noodle and oar. Speakers of _gua\spi_ should try to emulate this performance and to use foreign words only for truly local concepts, such as the ``|mu'' (a Chinese area unit for land) or ``adobo'' (a Filipino chicken stew). +When foreign words fit neatly into the language, speakers are tempted to over-use them. Agglutinative languages like _gua\spi_ and _Loglan_ have great power to produce compound words with quite precise meanings, and speakers should try very hard to learn to produce such words. It is a fact that despite _Loglan_ 's limited set of nouns and, at that time, its lack of foreign words, I was able to write about 20,000 words of text while inventing only four unapproved primitive words (and a fifth was made for me after the fact): bear, torus, tape, noodle and oar. Speakers of _gua\spi_ should try to emulate this performance and to use foreign words only for truly local concepts, such as the |mu (a Chinese area unit for land) or adobo (a Filipino chicken stew). See the discussion under [Pronouns: Foreign Words](../pronouns.html#c-foreign) about attaching foreign words to _gua\spi_ predicates to provide cases. ### Retroactive Downjumps -Sentences are usually connected by a retroactive downjump _``^:o''_. With this operator the preceeding sentence is taken out of the discourse and is inserted in the first case of the word following _``^:o''_ (which will need a default _``vo''_). Thus the following two sentences are equivalent: +Sentences are usually connected by a retroactive downjump _^:o_. With this operator the preceeding sentence is taken out of the discourse and is inserted in the first case of the word following _^:o_ (which will need a default _vo_). Thus the following two sentences are equivalent: > @@ -1215,15 +1215,15 @@ Sentences are usually connected by a retroactive downjump _``^:o''_. With this o -The form with the explicit infinitive is more natural in _gua\spi_. When retroactive downjumps are allowed you have to finish an entire sentence structure and hear the next word, possibly a downjump, before you can place the structure in the parse tree. However, all natural languages allow afterthought sentence connectives as in the above examples, and speakers rarely use forethought forms where the beginning of the antecedent sentence is marked. I wonder if users might resist an absolute requirement to put _``!vo -ge''_ at the beginning of the antecedent sentence. +The form with the explicit infinitive is more natural in _gua\spi_. When retroactive downjumps are allowed you have to finish an entire sentence structure and hear the next word, possibly a downjump, before you can place the structure in the parse tree. However, all natural languages allow afterthought sentence connectives as in the above examples, and speakers rarely use forethought forms where the beginning of the antecedent sentence is marked. I wonder if users might resist an absolute requirement to put _!vo -ge_ at the beginning of the antecedent sentence. Speakers, please try to minimize the use of retroactive downjumps. We shall see if it is feasible to outlaw them completely. -Since the antecedent sentence starts out at the top level, it is asserted by default and remains thus after the downjump. The consequent sentence is an ordinary infinitive, which is not asserted. If you wish to assert it, or the antecedent when not using the retroactive downjump, put in a _``ge''_ prefix as shown for the explicit infinitive antecedent. +Since the antecedent sentence starts out at the top level, it is asserted by default and remains thus after the downjump. The consequent sentence is an ordinary infinitive, which is not asserted. If you wish to assert it, or the antecedent when not using the retroactive downjump, put in a _ge_ prefix as shown for the explicit infinitive antecedent. -A sentence start word _``^:u''_ connects sentences with coordinated arguments, and such a group acts as a unit for retroactive downjumps. _``^:e''_ also makes such units, but the arguments are not coordinated. More complicated groupings are best handled by making explicit infinitive arguments out of the component sentences and using _``ge''_ to assert them, or by assigning names to the sentences and subsequently asserting a causal relation among the antecedents of the names. +A sentence start word _^:u_ connects sentences with coordinated arguments, and such a group acts as a unit for retroactive downjumps. _^:e_ also makes such units, but the arguments are not coordinated. More complicated groupings are best handled by making explicit infinitive arguments out of the component sentences and using _ge_ to assert them, or by assigning names to the sentences and subsequently asserting a causal relation among the antecedents of the names. -The retroactive downjump _``fy''_ relocates the sub-phrase before it at the same level, to become the first case of the following predicate at the same level. It roughly translates the infix ``and'' that connects a list of arguments in English. For example, +The retroactive downjump _fy_ relocates the sub-phrase before it at the same level, to become the first case of the following predicate at the same level. It roughly translates the infix and that connects a list of arguments in English. For example, > @@ -1276,13 +1276,13 @@ The retroactive downjump _``fy''_ relocates the sub-phrase before it at the same -This is one way to do multiple arguments (see also the section [just below](../vocab1.html#conjunctions)), and is just about the only use for _``fy''_ \--- but difficult for hard-core English speakers to give up. Needless to say, the retroactive downjump is not mandatory. ``_tla_ -set'' is used when the arguments form an unordered collection, while ``_stl_ -list'' is for arguments that have a sequence. Both words take as many arguments as you wish. +This is one way to do multiple arguments (see also the section [just below](../vocab1.html#conjunctions)), and is just about the only use for _fy_ \--- but difficult for hard-core English speakers to give up. Needless to say, the retroactive downjump is not mandatory. _tla_ -set is used when the arguments form an unordered collection, while _stl_ -list is for arguments that have a sequence. Both words take as many arguments as you wish. -When a sentence is complicated, one can use ``_fl_ -begin'' and ``_fr_ -end'' to mark the beginning and end of a grammatical unit. A matching pair of _``fl''_ and _``fr''_ are supposed to be at the same grammatical level; if they are not, someone has made an error. They have no meaning beyond this checking function. +When a sentence is complicated, one can use _fl_ -begin and _fr_ -end to mark the beginning and end of a grammatical unit. A matching pair of _fl_ and _fr_ are supposed to be at the same grammatical level; if they are not, someone has made an error. They have no meaning beyond this checking function. ### Error Correction -Another organizational transformation related to the retroactive downjump is error correction with _``fa''_. When prefixed to a phrase, it causes the previous phrase at the same level to vanish and to be replaced by the one that follows. _``/fi -fa''_ is a quick way to delete the current sentence. For example, +Another organizational transformation related to the retroactive downjump is error correction with _fa_. When prefixed to a phrase, it causes the previous phrase at the same level to vanish and to be replaced by the one that follows. _/fi -fa_ is a quick way to delete the current sentence. For example, > @@ -1322,7 +1322,7 @@ Another organizational transformation related to the retroactive downjump is err ### Conjunctions, Mixtures and Masses -The conjunction _``fe''_ has several functions depending on what it joins. First, when it joins phrase predicates it makes a collection of phrases with the same arguments; usually it is unseen in this function since the first tone `-' is usually sufficient to cue a parallel phrase compound. +The conjunction _fe_ has several functions depending on what it joins. First, when it joins phrase predicates it makes a collection of phrases with the same arguments; usually it is unseen in this function since the first tone `-' is usually sufficient to cue a parallel phrase compound. > @@ -1382,7 +1382,7 @@ The conjunction _``fe''_ has several functions depending on what it joins. First Despite our monkeylike eagerness to treat these words as units, we should remember that in _gua\spi_ there are phrases being conjoined. Thus everyone is told to go from the water _and_ to be out of the water; and the result of cutting is both a hole _and_ goes through the steel; the referent of this argument satisfies two predicates. -_``fe''_ also can join arguments. The first of the following examples shows a conjunction with _``fe''_ in which successive arguments are joined into one. (For details, see [Semantics: Cartesian Expansion](../semantic.html#S-conjunction).) The second example shows the same sentence rendered with ``_tla_ -set'' in which the set members are listed. _``fe''_ produces referents only in extension whereas _``tla''_ produces a set in suitable context. There is a similar word ``_stl_ -list'' when the arguments must have a specific order. For both words you may give as many arguments as you need. +_fe_ also can join arguments. The first of the following examples shows a conjunction with _fe_ in which successive arguments are joined into one. (For details, see [Semantics: Cartesian Expansion](../semantic.html#S-conjunction).) The second example shows the same sentence rendered with _tla_ -set in which the set members are listed. _fe_ produces referents only in extension whereas _tla_ produces a set in suitable context. There is a similar word _stl_ -list when the arguments must have a specific order. For both words you may give as many arguments as you need. > @@ -1418,7 +1418,7 @@ _``fe''_ also can join arguments. The first of the following examples shows a co -Here is a particularly troublesome example of ``or'' in a set of arguments: +Here is a particularly troublesome example of or in a set of arguments: > @@ -1504,9 +1504,9 @@ Mixtures are expressed in several ways. -_Loglan_ has a concept of a ``mass individual''. According to Brown [L1] it is more characteristic of non-Western cultures. Here is my best explanation of it. Take the full referent set of an argument, and personify it so that, potentially at least, it is the same kind of thing as its members. For example, all sharks can be considered to be instances or manifestations of an archetypical shark god. This composite object is the mass individual. In _Loglan_ , arguments in the ``serving or portion'' category, like ``_cutri_ -water'', generally are used as mass individuals. +_Loglan_ has a concept of a mass individual. According to Brown [L1] it is more characteristic of non-Western cultures. Here is my best explanation of it. Take the full referent set of an argument, and personify it so that, potentially at least, it is the same kind of thing as its members. For example, all sharks can be considered to be instances or manifestations of an archetypical shark god. This composite object is the mass individual. In _Loglan_ , arguments in the serving or portion category, like _cutri_ -water, generally are used as mass individuals. -Concepts and features which in other languages seem unitary are revealed in _gua\spi_ to be various. The mixtures above are one example, and mass individuals are another. Here are some examples related to mass individuals. Note that _``xo''_ is the default first case article for servings and portions and so in argument sites with the normal default you need not say _``xo''_. +Concepts and features which in other languages seem unitary are revealed in _gua\spi_ to be various. The mixtures above are one example, and mass individuals are another. Here are some examples related to mass individuals. Note that _xo_ is the default first case article for servings and portions and so in argument sites with the normal default you need not say _xo_. > @@ -1618,7 +1618,11 @@ Concepts and features which in other languages seem unitary are revealed in _gua @@ -1642,9 +1646,9 @@ Concepts and features which in other languages seem unitary are revealed in _gua -``Moby Dick'' is a bit ambiguous; it names both the book and the whale in it. The fault lies with the author for using one name for two referents. In any case, it is obvious what a foreign name means when referring to an object or a person. In the second example we have two _gua\spi_ predicate names, conveniently all compound words so that they feel the same as the foreign names. But in the third example the predicate name is quite intricate, extends over three grammatical levels, and includes an imbedded name, ``C'', represented by a letteral. Nonetheless the principle is the same; _``qu''_ converts the following phrase into a name. +Moby Dick is a bit ambiguous; it names both the book and the whale in it. The fault lies with the author for using one name for two referents. In any case, it is obvious what a foreign name means when referring to an object or a person. In the second example we have two _gua\spi_ predicate names, conveniently all compound words so that they feel the same as the foreign names. But in the third example the predicate name is quite intricate, extends over three grammatical levels, and includes an imbedded name, C, represented by a letteral. Nonetheless the principle is the same; _qu_ converts the following phrase into a name. -The fourth example is perhaps the most difficult: the declaration of a title. The title is not part of the discourse but rather tells about it, hence it takes the form of a decoration with _``vi''_ in a nonsentence. With it the by-line appears in the usual form for setting a modal case default. +The fourth example is perhaps the most difficult: the declaration of a title. The title is not part of the discourse but rather tells about it, hence it takes the form of a decoration with _vi_ in a nonsentence. With it the by-line appears in the usual form for setting a modal case default. ### Modal Cases @@ -1833,7 +1837,10 @@ Here are examples of the most important modal cases. However, virtually any word Said - Kira, ``You're a monster'' + Kira, + + You're a monster + @@ -1862,7 +1869,10 @@ Here are examples of the most important modal cases. However, virtually any word Thought - Kira, ``The pilot needs to be rescued'' + Kira, + + The pilot needs to be rescued + @@ -2174,11 +2184,11 @@ Here are examples of the most important modal cases. However, virtually any word
= (subset of Word, e.g. - ``vo'' + + vo + or - ``zu'' + + zu + ) = (subset of Word, e.g. - ``tara'' + + tara + or - ``crw'' + + crw + ) ; These add the effect of - ``fi'' + + fi + . = `^' (choice of words such as - ``:i'' + + :i + ) - ``Shit'' + + Shit + is unsuitable to be said at school He/she cried, - ``I want (to be in contact with) my mother'' + + I want (to be in contact with) my mother +
Captain Kirk said, - ``C'est la vie'' + + C'est la vie +
Hamlet said, - ``To be or not to be . . .'' + + To be or not to be . . . +
- Captain Kirk said to Helmsman Sulu, ``Activate warp engines'' + Captain Kirk said to Helmsman Sulu, + + Activate warp engines +
= (subset of Word, e.g. - ``vo'' + + vo + or - ``zu'' + + zu + ) = (subset of Word, e.g. - ``tara'' + + tara + or - ``crw'' + + crw + ) ; These add the effect of - ``fi'' + + fi + . = `^' (choice of words such as - ``:i'' + + :i + ) - (% represents `a' in ``cat'') + (% represents `a' in + + cat + + )
- Said Kira, ``Why do you flee?'' (default saved, set) + Said Kira, + + Why do you flee? + + (default saved, set)
- ``Don't you want to be rescued?'' (default inserted automatically) + + Don't you want to be rescued? + + (default inserted automatically)
- ``Please, at least talk to me!'' (default inserted automatically) + + Please, at least talk to me! + + (default inserted automatically)
His garment; which joins him to . . . ( - ``jy'' + + jy + = referent placeholder) - The ``C'' Programming Language + The + + C + + Programming Language was written by Kernighan and Ritchie
- There are several moderately complex cases, such as ``_tue_ -culture'', and ``_fta_ -standard for judgement'', which are relevant to only four or five words each. These are served by modal cases as a matter of policy, rather than having numbered cases on each word. + There are several moderately complex cases, such as _tue_ -culture, and _fta_ -standard for judgement, which are relevant to only four or five words each. These are served by modal cases as a matter of policy, rather than having numbered cases on each word. ### Tenses and Aspects -In English and all Indo-European languages, every sentence has a tense, that is, the syntax indicates (with great complexity) when the sentence occurred. Stories, for example, frequently have every narrative sentence in the past tense. _Gua\spi_ uses instead the modal case default for tense, and explicit tense modal cases appear only for sentences off the default. The sentence start word _``^:a''_ links sentences that occur in sequence. Here are some example sentences with tenses. +In English and all Indo-European languages, every sentence has a tense, that is, the syntax indicates (with great complexity) when the sentence occurred. Stories, for example, frequently have every narrative sentence in the past tense. _Gua\spi_ uses instead the modal case default for tense, and explicit tense modal cases appear only for sentences off the default. The sentence start word _^:a_ links sentences that occur in sequence. Here are some example sentences with tenses. > @@ -2266,7 +2276,7 @@ In English and all Indo-European languages, every sentence has a tense, that is, -In Russian, every verb is formally assigned an ``aspect'': ``perfective'', meaning that the sentence's event is considered as a unit, including its completion, and ``imperfective'', meaning that the predicated relation is continuous. English has these aspects too, though each verb can have either aspect depending on a moderately complicated syntactic cue (``ing'' for imperfective and various others for perfective). Speakers also like to distinguish a ``completed'' versus ``aborted'' aspect, whether an event reached its usual conclusion. In _gua\spi_ the unmodified predicates are perfective or imperfective according to their meanings, but subordinate clauses or compounding words can express whatever aspects are necessary. Here are a few aspects in _gua\spi_ : +In Russian, every verb is formally assigned an aspect: perfective, meaning that the sentence's event is considered as a unit, including its completion, and imperfective, meaning that the predicated relation is continuous. English has these aspects too, though each verb can have either aspect depending on a moderately complicated syntactic cue (ing for imperfective and various others for perfective). Speakers also like to distinguish a completed versus aborted aspect, whether an event reached its usual conclusion. In _gua\spi_ the unmodified predicates are perfective or imperfective according to their meanings, but subordinate clauses or compounding words can express whatever aspects are necessary. Here are a few aspects in _gua\spi_ : > @@ -2357,7 +2367,7 @@ In Russian, every verb is formally assigned an ``aspect'': ``perfective'', meani ### Decorations -A decoration is a short subordinate clause. Sometimes it expresses the speaker's attitude about the sentence or the relation between sentences, in which case its prefix is _``vi''_ , or it can be a subordinate assertion with _``va''_ , or an actual part of the main assertion, with _``vu''_ or `|' tone. +A decoration is a short subordinate clause. Sometimes it expresses the speaker's attitude about the sentence or the relation between sentences, in which case its prefix is _vi_ , or it can be a subordinate assertion with _va_ , or an actual part of the main assertion, with _vu_ or `|' tone. diff --git a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/vocab2.md b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/vocab2.md index 7e904d2a7..4e578caeb 100644 --- a/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/vocab2.md +++ b/data/pages/en/resources/guaspi/vocab2.md @@ -37,7 +37,7 @@ Even uneducated speakers quantify phrases, that is, they say how many or how big ### Numbers, Expressions and Functions -Cardinal numbers (here exemplified by _``cu''_ \- set of two) are defined as ``X1 is a set containing so many members X2''. The converted predicate means ``X2 is a member of a set X1 of so many members''. Quantifiers are subordinate clauses on an argument, e.g. +Cardinal numbers (here exemplified by _cu_ \- set of two) are defined as X1 is a set containing so many members X2. The converted predicate means ``X2 is a member of a set X1 of so many members''. Quantifiers are subordinate clauses on an argument, e.g. > @@ -63,7 +63,7 @@ Cardinal numbers (here exemplified by _``cu''_ \- set of two) are defined as ``X -How do you say ``the number two''? Any set with two members can be put in 1-1 correspondence with any other such set, but not with a set with different count; this forms an equivalence relation that segregates sets by count. Among the ways to define ``the number two'' the one that fits best in _gua\spi_ is _``xu -cu''_ , designating this equivalence class. All kinds of mathematical objects, such as rational, real, complex and dimensioned numbers, can be produced by various extension maneuvers from these equivalence classes, and can be named in _gua\spi_ by _``xu -N''_. +How do you say the number two? Any set with two members can be put in 1-1 correspondence with any other such set, but not with a set with different count; this forms an equivalence relation that segregates sets by count. Among the ways to define the number two the one that fits best in _gua\spi_ is _xu -cu_ , designating this equivalence class. All kinds of mathematical objects, such as rational, real, complex and dimensioned numbers, can be produced by various extension maneuvers from these equivalence classes, and can be named in _gua\spi_ by _xu -N_. > @@ -80,7 +80,11 @@ How do you say ``the number two''? Any set with two members can be put in 1-1 co - The number 2.5 (the class of all sets of ``count'' 2.5) + The number 2.5 (the class of all sets of + + count + + 2.5) @@ -89,7 +93,7 @@ How do you say ``the number two''? Any set with two members can be put in 1-1 co -Mathematical functions are defined with such classes as formal parameters, and hence have _``xu''_ on parameter cases by default --- _``xu''_ means the entire referent set of an argument, as a set (or class). The first case of a function is its value, and the function is defined as ``X1 is in the equivalence class that comes from doing (function) on (_xu_) X2'', possibly with several parameters. Thus a function can be used to predicate that something has a particular count or measure. _``xu''_ recovers the equivalence class. The abbreviation ``IEC'', meaning ``in equivalence class'', is used thus: ``X1 IEC the result of (whatever)''. For example, +Mathematical functions are defined with such classes as formal parameters, and hence have _xu_ on parameter cases by default --- _xu_ means the entire referent set of an argument, as a set (or class). The first case of a function is its value, and the function is defined as ``X1 is in the equivalence class that comes from doing (function) on (_xu_) X2'', possibly with several parameters. Thus a function can be used to predicate that something has a particular count or measure. _xu_ recovers the equivalence class. The abbreviation IEC, meaning in equivalence class, is used thus: X1 IEC the result of (whatever). For example, > @@ -118,11 +122,11 @@ Mathematical functions are defined with such classes as formal parameters, and h This syntax for mathematical expressions is neat, compact and unambiguous. No special syntax needs to be added to _gua\spi_ beyond that already in use for ordinary arguments and sentences. -Functions always deliver their value in the first case and take arguments in the second and following cases. For the range and domain of a function F, use _``xu -F''_ and _``xu -zu -F''_ respectively. +Functions always deliver their value in the first case and take arguments in the second and following cases. For the range and domain of a function F, use _xu -F_ and _xu -zu -F_ respectively. ### Ordinal Numbers and List Ends -An ordinal number, cued by the quasidigit _``tr''_ , means ``X1 is N'th in list (_xy_) X2 starting at X3''. For example: +An ordinal number, cued by the quasidigit _tr_ , means ``X1 is N'th in list (_xy_) X2 starting at X3''. For example: > @@ -148,7 +152,7 @@ An ordinal number, cued by the quasidigit _``tr''_ , means ``X1 is N'th in list -List ends and segments are built with ``_bny_ -begin'' and ``_fne_ -end'' restricted by a numeric predicate. Note the definition, ``X1 is the next or previous member of (xy) X2 after X3''; restrict with a numeric predicate to change to the N'th next or previous member. Without X3 the list ends are produced, but don't be confused by the polarity: ``_bny_ -next also means ``beginning'' or ``least'' when the list is ordered by size or degree; ``_fne_ -previous'' means ``end'' or ``most''. It is clearer to use an ordinal number when you can. For example, +List ends and segments are built with _bny_ -begin and _fne_ -end restricted by a numeric predicate. Note the definition, ``X1 is the next or previous member of (xy) X2 after X3''; restrict with a numeric predicate to change to the N'th next or previous member. Without X3 the list ends are produced, but don't be confused by the polarity: _bny_ -next also means ``beginning or least when the list is ordered by size or degree; _fne_ -previous means end or most. It is clearer to use an ordinal number when you can. For example, > @@ -217,11 +221,11 @@ List ends and segments are built with ``_bny_ -begin'' and ``_fne_ -end'' restri -Lists are ordered with smaller or negative numbers first, so the ``smallest'' is _``bny -sty -kqa''_ whereas the largest would be _``fne -sty -kqa''_ or, sorting the list in reverse order, _``/bny !sty -spl''_. See also the discussion of ``_sym_ -chief'' under [Comparative and Superlative](../vocab2.html#sym-chief) for a better way to do ``second smallest'' and the like. +Lists are ordered with smaller or negative numbers first, so the smallest is _bny -sty -kqa_ whereas the largest would be _fne -sty -kqa_ or, sorting the list in reverse order, _/bny !sty -spl_. See also the discussion of _sym_ -chief under [Comparative and Superlative](../vocab2.html#sym-chief) for a better way to do second smallest and the like. ### Vectors, Dates and Times -You express a vector as a ``_stl_ -list'' of expressions. Units of measure applied to a vector multiply each component individually. A matrix (by components) is a list of vectors, and so on. A date or time is also a list of expressions. +You express a vector as a _stl_ -list of expressions. Units of measure applied to a vector multiply each component individually. A matrix (by components) is a list of vectors, and so on. A date or time is also a list of expressions. > @@ -277,7 +281,7 @@ You express a vector as a ``_stl_ -list'' of expressions. Units of measure appli -The date is defined as ``X1 is the date of event (vo) X2+ starting with unit (xu-jani) X3* in calendar X4'' in which auto-conversion lets it restrict a sentence directly, while the unit can still be compounded. The first vector component has that unit, and subsequent components are multiplied by sub-units in the order years, months, days, hours, minutes, seconds. The default unit is ``_jani_ -years''. +The date is defined as ``X1 is the date of event (vo) X2+ starting with unit (xu-jani) X3* in calendar X4'' in which auto-conversion lets it restrict a sentence directly, while the unit can still be compounded. The first vector component has that unit, and subsequent components are multiplied by sub-units in the order years, months, days, hours, minutes, seconds. The default unit is _jani_ -years. ### Units of Measure @@ -318,11 +322,11 @@ Units of measure are defined to multiply a number or other expression by the uni -``Scientific notation'' is used in _gua\spi_ instead of the thousands and millions typical of English and in place of the metric prefixes; it is more compact and much easier to specify syntactically. +Scientific notation is used in _gua\spi_ instead of the thousands and millions typical of English and in place of the metric prefixes; it is more compact and much easier to specify syntactically. This definition of a unit is reasonable mathematically since a physical unit of measure can be interpreted as a basis member of a 1-dimensional vector space of things having that dimension. For example, consider mass. Take the set of all things with mass, and take equivalence classes of things with equal mass. Those equivalence classes occupy, and can be extended to create, a 1-D vector space. Any single member is a basis, and a unit is a member selected by convention, e.g. _the_ standard kilogram. Now for the word, its referent could be the unit, but you have to multiply it by the number (e.g. 2.5 times grams), which makes expressions too wordy. So the unit word is defined as a math function that multiplies by the unit. -In units of measure, the first argument occupants are not things but properties, e.g. masses of things, which are events, e.g. ``something is massive''. The need for a predicate to go with the thing being measured is easiest to see in 3-D, e.g. the argument could be high, wide or deep but all are measured by the single dimension of meters. Then the unit becomes a modal case of the predicate. These examples show how to use MKS and provincial units: +In units of measure, the first argument occupants are not things but properties, e.g. masses of things, which are events, e.g. something is massive. The need for a predicate to go with the thing being measured is easiest to see in 3-D, e.g. the argument could be high, wide or deep but all are measured by the single dimension of meters. Then the unit becomes a modal case of the predicate. These examples show how to use MKS and provincial units: > @@ -358,9 +362,9 @@ In units of measure, the first argument occupants are not things but properties, -In particular, no quantifiable relation (e.g. ``heavy'' or ``exceeds in dimension _vo_ X3'') has an explicit case for how much it is, relying instead on the modal case of units. There is one exception: ``_kun_ -quantity'' is like a unit in providing a modal case for quantity, but provides an identity transformation, so that a question word can be dropped into the multiplicand argument without forcing a specific unit. +In particular, no quantifiable relation (e.g. heavy or ``exceeds in dimension _vo_ X3'') has an explicit case for how much it is, relying instead on the modal case of units. There is one exception: _kun_ -quantity is like a unit in providing a modal case for quantity, but provides an identity transformation, so that a question word can be dropped into the multiplicand argument without forcing a specific unit. -To talk about the unit rather than to use it, use _``xu vo ''_, as in ``the pound is a provincial unit''. _``xe vo ''_ will deliver the standard unit, if there is one, given suitable context cues. +To talk about the unit rather than to use it, use _``xu vo ''_, as in the pound is a provincial unit. _xe vo _ will deliver the standard unit, if there is one, given suitable context cues. Compound units, like ohms, require a product or quotient of several units. One may use the personal name units (ohm, volt, pascal, celsius) in the same manner as provincial units. @@ -700,27 +704,27 @@ Compound units, like ohms, require a product or quotient of several units. One m _^:i _-sfa_ !kio !ji ^tara |zey !ju_ _It is false_ that I have your rat. This is the prototype of negation, and it is the policy in _gua\spi_ to use predicates when possible rather than prefixes or other structure words. However, the negated sentence is an extra level down, a problem for speakers. _^:i _-go_ !ji /kio !tara |zey !ju_ - I _don't_ have your rat. _``go''_ is a mood prefix which means that the asserted sentence is counter to fact. It is simpler and more familiar to natural language speakers than ``_sfa_ -false'' is, and it works in subordinate clauses where _``sfa''_ doesn't. + I _don't_ have your rat. _go_ is a mood prefix which means that the asserted sentence is counter to fact. It is simpler and more familiar to natural language speakers than _sfa_ -false is, and it works in subordinate clauses where _sfa_ doesn't. _^:i !ji /kio !tara _|go_ -zey !ju_ - I have a rat which _isn't_ yours. _``go''_ can equally be used in subordinate clauses, or even in argument predicates. + I have a rat which _isn't_ yours. _go_ can equally be used in subordinate clauses, or even in argument predicates. _^:i !ji /kio _!xn_ -kseo_ - I have _no_ cheese. _``xn''_ means that of the members of the full referent set of the argument, none fit in the predicated relation. Unlike the rest of the articles, this is actually a statement about the excluded members, and means the same as _``^:i !ji _/go_ -kio !xa -kseo''_ \--- freely translated, ``for all pieces of cheese, I don't have it''. (See De Morgan's rules below.) + I have _no_ cheese. _xn_ means that of the members of the full referent set of the argument, none fit in the predicated relation. Unlike the rest of the articles, this is actually a statement about the excluded members, and means the same as _^:i !ji _/go_ -kio !xa -kseo_ \--- freely translated, ``for all pieces of cheese, I don't have it''. (See De Morgan's rules below.) _^:i !ji /kio !kseo _|zu -cy__ - I have _zero_ pieces of cheese. This is the most natural form of argument negation in _Loglan_ , but _gua\spi_ looks strictly at referent sets, and if you say you have all the members of the null set, it isn't a cheesy null set --- there is only one null set. The statement is a tautology, and says nothing about cheese. Many logical fallacies, such as St. Anselm's ontological proof of the existence of God, are like this example in that they prove a statement about the members of a set which may not have any members. In _gua\spi_ use _``xn''_ as above. + I have _zero_ pieces of cheese. This is the most natural form of argument negation in _Loglan_ , but _gua\spi_ looks strictly at referent sets, and if you say you have all the members of the null set, it isn't a cheesy null set --- there is only one null set. The statement is a tautology, and says nothing about cheese. Many logical fallacies, such as St. Anselm's ontological proof of the existence of God, are like this example in that they prove a statement about the members of a set which may not have any members. In _gua\spi_ use _xn_ as above. _^:i !ji /kio _!ple_ !tara_ - I have something which _isn't_ the rat. The full referent set of _``ple !xe -tara''_ (and therefore its referent subset) is in the complement of the referent subset of _``xe -tara''_. + I have something which _isn't_ the rat. The full referent set of _ple !xe -tara_ (and therefore its referent subset) is in the complement of the referent subset of _xe -tara_. _^:i !jw |kseo /fi _-stu_ -zao_ - This cheese is _bad_ in flavor. In George Orwell's 1984, the language ``newspeak'' was designed to destroy the ability of people to think, and one of its design features was that negative words were eliminated; ``bad'' became ``ungood''. _Gua\spi_ (imitating _Loglan_) offers specific negated words for major predicates when the negations are used frequently. Nonetheless, most negations will have to be done with compound words as in the next examples. Be alert for creative expression possibilities such as _``^:i !jw |kseo !fu _-zu -dyi_ ''_ \--- ``this cheese is _disgusting_ ''. + This cheese is _bad_ in flavor. In George Orwell's 1984, the language newspeak was designed to destroy the ability of people to think, and one of its design features was that negative words were eliminated; bad became ungood. _Gua\spi_ (imitating _Loglan_) offers specific negated words for major predicates when the negations are used frequently. Nonetheless, most negations will have to be done with compound words as in the next examples. Be alert for creative expression possibilities such as _``^:i !jw |kseo !fu _-zu -dyi_ ''_ \--- this cheese is _disgusting_. _^:i !jw |kseo !fu _/gl_ -zao_ This cheese is flavor _less_. Many dimensions are quantifiable (more or less) but unsigned, so their degree ranges from zero to larger values. This is how to assert that the degree is zero or negligible. _^:i !jw |kseo !fu _/gr_ -ksi_ - This cheese is _not_ fresh. When the dimension ranges from positive to negative values, _``gr''_ interchanges positive and negative. On occasion, _``gl''_ will also apply to indicate the zero point, though it is meaningless with ``_ksi_ -fresh''. For extremes of unfreshness one can use ``_fpu_ -rotten''. + This cheese is _not_ fresh. When the dimension ranges from positive to negative values, _gr_ interchanges positive and negative. On occasion, _gl_ will also apply to indicate the zero point, though it is meaningless with _ksi_ -fresh. For extremes of unfreshness one can use _fpu_ -rotten. _^:i !jw |kseo /fi _-vry_ -can -psl_ - This cheese is _de_ solidifying. When a process occurs in the reverse of the usual order, ``_vry_ -reverse'' indicates this. + This cheese is _de_ solidifying. When a process occurs in the reverse of the usual order, _vry_ -reverse indicates this. ### De Morgan's Rules in Quantification -Negation interacts with ``and'' and ``or'', which necessarily occur in sentences which are quantified or whose arguments have multiple referents. Therefore it is advisable to digress into some elementary symbolic logic. Here is De Morgan's rule for negation, stated four ways: (A and B are sentences) +Negation interacts with and and or, which necessarily occur in sentences which are quantified or whose arguments have multiple referents. Therefore it is advisable to digress into some elementary symbolic logic. Here is De Morgan's rule for negation, stated four ways: (A and B are sentences) > @@ -824,7 +828,7 @@ Negation interacts with ``and'' and ``or'', which necessarily occur in sentences -Remember that in logic, ``A or B'' is true if one or _both_ of the statements is true, unlike in English where the ``or'' generally excludes both being true. +Remember that in logic, A or B is true if one or _both_ of the statements is true, unlike in English where the or generally excludes both being true. Universal quantification means a statement is true when applied to all members of a set, of the form ``S1 and S2 and S3 and . . .'', where S1 is the statement applied to member 1 and so on. Existential quantification means that a statement is true about at least one set member, in form ``S1 or S2 or S3 or . . .'' When such statements are negated, De Morgan's rule applies. Here are some more specific examples. @@ -833,11 +837,11 @@ _^:i -kio !ji ^kseo_ _^:i -kio !ji ^kseo |zu -to_ I have at least one piece of cheese. Existential quantification like this means the same as ``I have piece 1 _or_ I have piece 2 _or_ . . .'' for all pieces of cheese. Now the simplest negation of this sentence is simply: _^:i -sfa !kio !ji ^kseo |zu -to_ - It is false that I have at least one piece of cheese. This form does not suit typical speakers; we want to negate the relation word ``_kio_ -possess'', not the whole sentence, like this: + It is false that I have at least one piece of cheese. This form does not suit typical speakers; we want to negate the relation word _kio_ -possess, not the whole sentence, like this: _^:i !ji _/go_ -kio !xa -kseo_ - I don't have any cheese --- I don't have piece 1 _and_ I don't have piece 2 _and_ . . . To negate (or de-negate) a disjunction (compound sentence with ``or''), we had to change ``or'' to ``and'', producing a universal quantification. The same principle applies when you start with a universal: + I don't have any cheese --- I don't have piece 1 _and_ I don't have piece 2 _and_ . . . To negate (or de-negate) a disjunction (compound sentence with or), we had to change or to and, producing a universal quantification. The same principle applies when you start with a universal: _^:i !xa -xe -cil /jir_ - All the children are here --- Child 1 is here _and_ child 2 is here _and_ . . . Rather than negating the whole sentence with ``_sfa_ -false'', let us negate the predicate ``_jir_ -here'': + All the children are here --- Child 1 is here _and_ child 2 is here _and_ . . . Rather than negating the whole sentence with _sfa_ -false, let us negate the predicate _jir_ -here: _^:i -go -jir !cil _|zu -to__ At least one of the children is not here --- Child 1 is not here _or_ child 2 is not here _or_ . . . In general, when you negate the predicate of a sentence involving quantification or multiple argument referents of any kind, you will also have to reverse the type of quantification or conjunction used. @@ -845,7 +849,7 @@ _^:i -go -jir !cil _|zu -to__ ### Moods and Imperatives -These are the mood prefixes in _gua\spi_ , which indicate the manner of assertion of a phrase. A top level sentence has _``ge''_ on it by default unless another mood prefix appears. +These are the mood prefixes in _gua\spi_ , which indicate the manner of assertion of a phrase. A top level sentence has _ge_ on it by default unless another mood prefix appears. > @@ -990,7 +994,7 @@ These are the mood prefixes in _gua\spi_ , which indicate the manner of assertio -Closely related to the mood prefixes is the aspect operator ``_tri_ -ritual'', a sign of a performative phrase. ``Performative'' means that by uttering the words the speaker makes something true, as in a marriage vow or the illustrated naming ceremony. Note that auto-conversion is suppressed by _``zo''_ ; without it, the sentence would merely be the topic of a ceremony, not the ceremony itself. +Closely related to the mood prefixes is the aspect operator _tri_ -ritual, a sign of a performative phrase. Performative means that by uttering the words the speaker makes something true, as in a marriage vow or the illustrated naming ceremony. Note that auto-conversion is suppressed by _zo_ ; without it, the sentence would merely be the topic of a ceremony, not the ceremony itself. > @@ -1020,7 +1024,7 @@ Closely related to the mood prefixes is the aspect operator ``_tri_ -ritual'', a -In English there is an imperative mood; however, in _gua\spi_ you make a sentence imperative by using ``_jo_ -you'' or ``_ja_ -we'' in the case for the actor, generally the first. These pronouns are distinguished from the non-imperative ``_ju_ -you'' and ``_je_ -we''. A decoration ``_pli_ -please'' softens the command. For example, +In English there is an imperative mood; however, in _gua\spi_ you make a sentence imperative by using _jo_ -you or _ja_ -we in the case for the actor, generally the first. These pronouns are distinguished from the non-imperative _ju_ -you and _je_ -we. A decoration _pli_ -please softens the command. For example, > @@ -1058,9 +1062,9 @@ In English there is an imperative mood; however, in _gua\spi_ you make a sentenc ### Special Features of Infinitives -In an infinitive the previous argument is replicated by default as the infinitive's first argument, while the first argument of a subordinate clause comes normally from the restricted phrase. Hence numbered cases skip over the first argument, and you must use the caselink _``so''_ for any explicit first case in an infinitive or subordinate clause. In an infinitive with _``vo''_ a predicate is made out of the sub-sentence that follows, including arguments and clauses. In the rare case where a sub-phrase (like a subordinate clause) must go on the infinitive predicate rather than into the sub-sentence, you can put a prefix before _``vo''_ , like an article, and put the clause between the article and _``vo''_. +In an infinitive the previous argument is replicated by default as the infinitive's first argument, while the first argument of a subordinate clause comes normally from the restricted phrase. Hence numbered cases skip over the first argument, and you must use the caselink _so_ for any explicit first case in an infinitive or subordinate clause. In an infinitive with _vo_ a predicate is made out of the sub-sentence that follows, including arguments and clauses. In the rare case where a sub-phrase (like a subordinate clause) must go on the infinitive predicate rather than into the sub-sentence, you can put a prefix before _vo_ , like an article, and put the clause between the article and _vo_. -When an infinitive with _``vo''_ is an argument, the main sentence asserts the relation of arguments to the infinitive's events, but does not make a separate assertion of those events. To additionally assert or deny the sub-phrase, use _``ge''_ or _``go''_ respectively. For example: +When an infinitive with _vo_ is an argument, the main sentence asserts the relation of arguments to the infinitive's events, but does not make a separate assertion of those events. To additionally assert or deny the sub-phrase, use _ge_ or _go_ respectively. For example: > @@ -1147,7 +1151,7 @@ Natural languages have various complicated arrangements to change a simple prope -In the case of ``_sym_ -superlative'' it is possible for several members to be equally green, each being greener than the remaining members. Also, a numeric predicate modifying _``sym''_ produces the N'th greenest member. Here are some sentences with comparatives and superlatives: +In the case of _sym_ -superlative it is possible for several members to be equally green, each being greener than the remaining members. Also, a numeric predicate modifying _sym_ produces the N'th greenest member. Here are some sentences with comparatives and superlatives: > @@ -1212,21 +1216,21 @@ In the case of ``_sym_ -superlative'' it is possible for several members to be e _^:i !tara /crw !kseo ^:o _-kau_ !gai -tuol !kseo_ The rat eats the cheese, and that _causes_ it to be dirty. A cause is rather mechanical. Actors with free will are rarely caused to do anything, despite their protestations. Here the rat may have free will, but the cheese, caused to be dirty, certainly does not. _^:i !ji /gri !tara ^kai |kei ^kseo ^:o _-kmo_ !qma -qtu !ji ^tara_ - I am angry at the rat for stealing the cheese, which _motivates_ me to kill the rat. The theft motivates the anger and the anger motivates the planned killing. When a free agent acts it is usually because of a motivation. Here the speaker includes ``_kei_ -crime'' in the sentence as a justification for his action. The definition of this word reminds you that it has the modal case ``_tue_ -culture'', which presumably includes the speaker --- but not the rat. + I am angry at the rat for stealing the cheese, which _motivates_ me to kill the rat. The theft motivates the anger and the anger motivates the planned killing. When a free agent acts it is usually because of a motivation. Here the speaker includes _kei_ -crime in the sentence as a justification for his action. The definition of this word reminds you that it has the modal case _tue_ -culture, which presumably includes the speaker --- but not the rat. _^:i !xi -tara /qai -crw |jro ^kseo ^:o _-zu -zni_ !vel !klo ^kseo_ - So that rats cannot eat the cheese, is the _reason_ the cheese is in a closed container. A reason is an end (ending event) or consequence that _motivates_ someone to make a starting event happen, such as keeping the cheese in the box, that will _cause_ the consequence. The concept of ``_zni_ -reason'' is rather slippery. First, the desired or planned consequence should be stated, not its inverse; _``^:i !xo -tara /gi -crw !kseo''_ = ``A rat might eat the cheese'' is the negative of the correct consequence. Second, we say in English ``past event Y is the reason for action Z'' where the _gua\spi_ definition of ``_zni_ -reason'' requires ``vengeance for past event Y'' --- a future consequence of action Z. _``vou''_ = ``vengeance''. Third, a ``_gul_ -rule'' can be said to _cause_ its reason, provided the obligees obey it. + So that rats cannot eat the cheese, is the _reason_ the cheese is in a closed container. A reason is an end (ending event) or consequence that _motivates_ someone to make a starting event happen, such as keeping the cheese in the box, that will _cause_ the consequence. The concept of _zni_ -reason is rather slippery. First, the desired or planned consequence should be stated, not its inverse; _``^:i !xo -tara /gi -crw !kseo''_ = A rat might eat the cheese is the negative of the correct consequence. Second, we say in English ``past event Y is the reason for action Z'' where the _gua\spi_ definition of _zni_ -reason requires vengeance for past event Y \--- a future consequence of action Z. _vou_ = vengeance. Third, a _gul_ -rule can be said to _cause_ its reason, provided the obligees obey it. _^:i -dae !kara ^:o _-sny_ !pwo -cyr -xyn !xi -tara ^kara_ - The box being open _implies_ that a rat can go into it. The relation of logical entailment has to do with definitions and theorems, not with the arrangement of the real world or the will of its actors. ``_zny_ -imply'' is the corresponding set operator: ``X1 is the union of X2 and the complement of X3'', where X2 and X3 can be infinitives with _``vo''_. Perhaps the distinction between _``sny''_ and _``zny''_ is merely an artifact of old _Loglan_ and English usage. We shall see if this is true as _gua\spi_ matures. + The box being open _implies_ that a rat can go into it. The relation of logical entailment has to do with definitions and theorems, not with the arrangement of the real world or the will of its actors. _zny_ -imply is the corresponding set operator: ``X1 is the union of X2 and the complement of X3'', where X2 and X3 can be infinitives with _vo_. Perhaps the distinction between _sny_ and _zny_ is merely an artifact of old _Loglan_ and English usage. We shall see if this is true as _gua\spi_ matures. _^:i -dae !kara ^:o _-bal_ !crw |jro ^tara ^kseo_ If the box is open _then maybe_ the rat will eat the cheese. This kind of fuzzy inference based on real-world consequences is what people use most often, rather than pure logic. _^:i !ji _/gu_ -fli ^:o _-sar !gu_ -vlw !ji ^qyun_ - _If_ I could fly I _would_ go to the moon. Necessary conditions are very commonly expressed and the logical ``if-then'' catches their true meaning poorly. Related is ``_sno_ -sufficient''. + _If_ I could fly I _would_ go to the moon. Necessary conditions are very commonly expressed and the logical if-then catches their true meaning poorly. Related is _sno_ -sufficient. These are the sentence connectives most often seen. But the speaker may connect sentences with any useful word having suitable cases. And like all _gua\spi_ words, the sentence connectives can also be useful as arguments and as modal caselinks. ### Logical Sentence Connectives -Old _Loglan_ was intended to be a ``logical language'', thereby to differ as much as possible from English. Therefore, one of its key features is support for what amounts to spoken symbolic logic. This feature is de-emphasized in _gua\spi_ ; in practice, what language users encounter most often, and stumble over, are Cartesian expansion of multiple arguments, non-commutative quantification, and complicated negations. These topics are well-supported in _gua\spi_. Nonetheless, set arithmetic can be performed on infinitives and the result is a set of events to which the listener's attention is drawn, just as with a more normal sentence. The logician's ``if-then'' can be realized through _``zny''_. Here are some examples of logical sentence connectives: +Old _Loglan_ was intended to be a logical language, thereby to differ as much as possible from English. Therefore, one of its key features is support for what amounts to spoken symbolic logic. This feature is de-emphasized in _gua\spi_ ; in practice, what language users encounter most often, and stumble over, are Cartesian expansion of multiple arguments, non-commutative quantification, and complicated negations. These topics are well-supported in _gua\spi_. Nonetheless, set arithmetic can be performed on infinitives and the result is a set of events to which the listener's attention is drawn, just as with a more normal sentence. The logician's if-then can be realized through _zny_. Here are some examples of logical sentence connectives: > @@ -1353,33 +1357,33 @@ Many abstract comparisons (1.1.1) and set member words (1.1.3) include a dimensi -``_xgi_ -green'' is applied to both X1 and X2 in the first and second sentence. This is described as a ``dual merge''. In the last sentence, ``_xgi_ -green'' is applied to X1 and to members of X2. The dictionary indicates all these special merges. +_xgi_ -green is applied to both X1 and X2 in the first and second sentence. This is described as a dual merge. In the last sentence, _xgi_ -green is applied to X1 and to members of X2. The dictionary indicates all these special merges. -``_stl_ -list'' involves a dimension which is applied pairwise to members of the list, indicating the ordering. +_stl_ -list involves a dimension which is applied pairwise to members of the list, indicating the ordering. -``_qaw_ -equally'' has a very unusual definition: the first case is an infinitive into whose first case the rest of the cases are copied in turn; the predicate means that all the arguments fit in the infinitive equally. Normally the predicate of this infinitive is provided by compounding, as in the example above. +_qaw_ -equally has a very unusual definition: the first case is an infinitive into whose first case the rest of the cases are copied in turn; the predicate means that all the arguments fit in the infinitive equally. Normally the predicate of this infinitive is provided by compounding, as in the example above. ### Sets -For several words in category 1.1.2 (sets) of the form ``(set) X1 is a (whatever)'', you can make a compound _``vdr =W''_ to get the members. +For several words in category 1.1.2 (sets) of the form ``(set) X1 is a (whatever)'', you can make a compound _vdr =W_ to get the members. -When _``xy''_ (in-mind set) is the default article for a case, then if the referents are sets the default changes automatically to _``xe''_ (in-mind in extension). But _``xu''_ (whole set) does not change to _``xa''_ (same in extension) because in math functions the usual occupant of such a case is supposed to be a set of equal-count sets. +When _xy_ (in-mind set) is the default article for a case, then if the referents are sets the default changes automatically to _xe_ (in-mind in extension). But _xu_ (whole set) does not change to _xa_ (same in extension) because in math functions the usual occupant of such a case is supposed to be a set of equal-count sets. -The predicates ``_tla_ -set'' and ``_stl_ -list'' have a special arrangement of cases. They mean ``X1 is a set (in extension) or list (ordered) consisting of members X2, X3, X4, . . .'', as many cases as needed. If X2 etc. have multiple referents in extension (which must be ordered for _``stl''_), all referents go in the set or list. Five or six words have this ``as many as needed'' argument list. +The predicates _tla_ -set and _stl_ -list have a special arrangement of cases. They mean ``X1 is a set (in extension) or list (ordered) consisting of members X2, X3, X4, . . .'', as many cases as needed. If X2 etc. have multiple referents in extension (which must be ordered for _stl_), all referents go in the set or list. Five or six words have this as many as needed argument list. ### Properties -Noncomparative Properties are distinguished in _Loglan_ from the Comparative Properties in that it is not useful to say that X is more than Y; for example, X is more dead than Y. For this reason _Loglan_ Comparative Properties each have a case for the compared item and Noncomparative Properties do not. Nonetheless, many of its members may actually be used comparatively (like ``_ksu_ -delicious'') and the distinction is rather artificial. In _gua\spi_ , Properties do not have comparative arguments. +Noncomparative Properties are distinguished in _Loglan_ from the Comparative Properties in that it is not useful to say that X is more than Y; for example, X is more dead than Y. For this reason _Loglan_ Comparative Properties each have a case for the compared item and Noncomparative Properties do not. Nonetheless, many of its members may actually be used comparatively (like _ksu_ -delicious) and the distinction is rather artificial. In _gua\spi_ , Properties do not have comparative arguments. -Directional Properties (1.5): These are often compounded with motion words, in which the moving case is related to the destination. (Special case: ``_tai_ -outside'' merges with the start point. Examples in ``Compound Words''.) Note that the polarity (e.g. up/down) in such compounds is often backwards from English. +Directional Properties (1.5): These are often compounded with motion words, in which the moving case is related to the destination. (Special case: _tai_ -outside merges with the start point. Examples in Compound Words.) Note that the polarity (e.g. up/down) in such compounds is often backwards from English. Timelike Directional Properties (1.5.3): These are the relation words for the tense modal case. ### Behaviors -Abstract Behaviors(2.1): These have the form ``X1 does (vo) X2+1'', in which X1 is automatically replicated as the first case of the infinitive _``vo X2''_. +Abstract Behaviors(2.1): These have the form X1 does o X2+1, in which X1 is automatically replicated as the first case of the infinitive _vo X2_. -Double Actor Transitive Activities (2.1.3): These have the prototype ``X1 makes X2 do (vo) X3+2'', in which X2 is automatically replicated as the first case of _``vo X3''_. +Double Actor Transitive Activities (2.1.3): These have the prototype X1 makes X2 do o X3+2, in which X2 is automatically replicated as the first case of _vo X3_. Games for Two Players (2.1.4): Generally you will want to use a reciprocal construction like this, unless the relation really is unilateral: @@ -1447,11 +1451,11 @@ Animals and Plants (3.1): These have just one argument. The animals and plants c Body Parts (3.2): These have the prototype ``X1 is a (part) of creature X2*''. -Materials (3.3): Almost all of these are of the form ``(xo) X1 is a serving/portion of (material)''. The _``xo''_ appears by default when the word is used as an argument, unless the containing sentence provides a default article other than the usual _``xe''_. +Materials (3.3): Almost all of these are of the form ``(xo) X1 is a serving/portion of (material)''. The _xo_ appears by default when the word is used as an argument, unless the containing sentence provides a default article other than the usual _xe_. Places, Seasons and Weather (3.5): Places mostly have the form ``X1 is a (place) of locality or superset X2''. -Containers (4.1.1) and Cooking and Eating (4.1.2): These have the form ``X1 is a container containing (xo) X2*''. Constructions like ``spoonful'' are handled with ``_ful_ -contained quantity'', like this: +Containers (4.1.1) and Cooking and Eating (4.1.2): These have the form X1 is a container containing (xo) X2*. Constructions like spoonful are handled with _ful_ -contained quantity, like this: > @@ -1479,17 +1483,17 @@ Containers (4.1.1) and Cooking and Eating (4.1.2): These have the form ``X1 is a Transport (4.1.4), Machines (4.1.5), and Parts of Structures (4.1.7): Many of these are like body parts: ``X1 is a (part) of structure X2*''. -Houses (4.1.8): House parts are as above. Houses themselves have the form ``X1 is a (house) of resident X2*''. +Houses (4.1.8): House parts are as above. Houses themselves have the form X1 is a (house) of resident X2*. -Cloth and Parts of Clothes (4.2.2): Parts are as above. Cloth has the form ``(xo) X1 is a portion of (cloth)''. +Cloth and Parts of Clothes (4.2.2): Parts are as above. Cloth has the form (xo) X1 is a portion of (cloth). Food (4.3): Mostly of the form ``(xo) X1 is a serving/portion of (food)''. -Works of Art (4.4.1): All have the form ``X1 is a (thing) about X2 created by X3 and performed by X4''. X2 may be an event or a thing; there is no _``vo''_ default. X4 is present only on relevant words such as ``_jiul_ -drama''. +Works of Art (4.4.1): All have the form ``X1 is a (thing) about X2 created by X3 and performed by X4''. X2 may be an event or a thing; there is no _vo_ default. X4 is present only on relevant words such as _jiul_ -drama. ### Miscellaneous Categories -Nationalities (4.7.1): _Loglan_ has words for nationalities, for the languages spoken there, and for the basis money unit of the nation. But only about fifteen arbitrarily chosen nations are supported, mainly European ones. _Gua\spi_ uses foreign names for these concepts, through ``_zina_ -nation'', ``_gua_ -language'', and ``_cni_ -money''. ``_spi_ -person'' translates the usual self-referential word in primitive languages for ethnic members of that culture. +Nationalities (4.7.1): _Loglan_ has words for nationalities, for the languages spoken there, and for the basis money unit of the nation. But only about fifteen arbitrarily chosen nations are supported, mainly European ones. _Gua\spi_ uses foreign names for these concepts, through _zina_ -nation, _gua_ -language, and _cni_ -money. _spi_ -person translates the usual self-referential word in primitive languages for ethnic members of that culture. Business (4.7.3): A number of these words have the form ``X1 (sells) goods or services X2 to other trader X3 for amount of money (xu) X4''. @@ -1655,7 +1659,10 @@ So far, the corpus of _gua\spi_ text available for analysis consists of 3140 wor 28 - Article ``any'' + Article + + any + @@ -1732,7 +1739,10 @@ So far, the corpus of _gua\spi_ text available for analysis consists of 3140 wor 17 - Article ``typical'' + Article + + typical + @@ -1787,7 +1797,10 @@ So far, the corpus of _gua\spi_ text available for analysis consists of 3140 wor 11 - Article ``all'' + Article + + all +